Thursday, May 15, 2025

Spelling to Communicate Goes on Trial: Part II

 

Spelling to Communicate Goes on Trial: Part II

This is the second part in a series on a Spelling to Communicate (S2C) lawsuit against a school district. You can read more about the background in my first post, but, in brief, the lawsuit arose because the school district, the Lower Merion School District of Lower Merion PA, refused to hire an S2C “communication partner” for the parents’ non-speaking autistic son (A.L.).

This series focuses, specifically, on a hearing that occurred last December that I now have permission to write about. The hearing was a Daubert hearing, aka a “voir dire” (an oral questioning/examination) of several of the plaintiff’s expert witnesses whom the School District sought to disqualify.

In my previous post, I described the examination (as opposed to cross-examination) of Dr. Anne Robbins, the neuropsychologist whom the parents hired to do an independent, neurocognitive assessment of A.L.: an assessment that was based, in part, on output that A.L. generated via S2C. The examination of Dr. Robbins was conducted by one of the plaintiff’s lawyers, Nicole Reimann (the only one of their lawyers in attendance at this hearing). Dr. Robbins’ testimony reports several psychologically extraordinary things about A.L.:

·         He has average or above listening comprehension capabilities but (on certain nonverbal subtests) didn’t understand what she was asking him to do.

·         He couldn’t point to a numbered choice on a stationary surface but could point to a number on a held-up letterboard.

·         His scores, based on S2C-generated output, were much higher on verbal subtests than on nonverbal visual pattern subtests: a striking reversal of what’s generally found in autism.

In this post, taking advantage of the public but pay-walled transcript I’ve acquired, I’m going to skip back in time to the morning of December 2nd, and to a part of the Daubert hearing that I didn’t attend: the examination of Dr. Wendy Ross. Dr. Ross, the director of Jefferson Health Center for Autism and Neurodiversity, is, as we’ll see in the course of this examination, an influential and highly celebrated figure in the Philadelphia autism and neurodiversity scene.

As before, I’ve put my commentary in bracketed italics and elided the parts of the examination that are more procedural in nature and/or less relevant to the questions that concern us here at FacilitatedCommunication.org. These questions are: what can we learn about the validity of the instances of S2C that have arisen in this case? And, in the case of the examination of Dr. Ross: what leads an accomplished medical professional who is highly experienced in autism to believe in the validity of S2C? And what sorts of justifications for this belief does this autism professional provide in response to friendly questioning while testifying under oath?

The examination begins with Ms. Reimann asking Dr. Ross how she knows A.L., referred to here as “Alex.”

Ross: Alex was my patient.

Reimann: Did he also participate in programs that your nonprofit developed?

Ross: Yes. So I had a practice, and I also had a nonprofit that worked on community participation. And many of my patients and other people from the autistic community participated in those experiences.

Reimann: And did Alex also serve at some point as an advisor for neurodiversity to give perspective as a person with autism?

Ross: Yes.

[Dr. Ross does not specify the means by which A.L. provided advice, but presumably it, like all the other verbal output that has been attributed to him, was generated via S2C.]

Reimann: And you are a developmental pediatrician, correct?

Ross: Yes.

Reimann: And what is the practice focus of a developmental pediatrician?

[There follows a lengthy discussion Dr. Ross’s educational background, which includes Mount Sinai, Yale New Haven Hospital, and Boston Children’s Hospital;  her initial practice at Albert Einstein Medical Center; and her current positions as Associate Professor at Jefferson University and Director of the Jefferson Health Center for Autism and Neurodiversity.

Also discussed is her work in school settings; her collaborative work with teachers, speech and language pathologists, and psychologists; and her work, in particular, with three individuals:

·         Neuropsychologist Dr. Anne Robbins (see above)

·         Speech-language pathologist Susan Chaplick, whom A.L.’s parents introduced to S2C several years ago and who accepts S2Ced output as valid, and

·         Child and adolescent psychiatrist Dr. Manley Ghaffari, who testified in an earlier hearing that the letter board was “extremely effective in allowing [Alex] to express his thoughts and feelings.”  

Dr. Ross characterizes all three individuals as having excellent reputations.

Discussed as well is Dr. Ross’s longstanding interest in autism and how 60-70% of her patients are autistic, totaling “probably thousands.” We learn about her nonprofit, Autism Inclusion Resources, which has worked to make museums, sporting events, and air travel more accessible for autism families. We learn that New Jersey Senator Lautenberg and then-transportation secretary Ray LaHood have each asked her to share her findings—e.g., with the Department of Transportation. We learn that she was selected as a top-ten CNN hero. We learn about how she partnered with the Special Olympics to get people with intellectual disabilities included among those prioritized for early COVID vaccination, leading to a publication in the New England Journal of Medicine Catalyst. And we learn about how she worked with the Pennsylvania court system to make family courts more autism friendly by educating lawyers, judges, and guardian ad litems about autism; by helping to design their sensory rooms; and by writing “visual stories” that could be used across the state with autistic individuals.

We learn that Dr. Ross spends 60 or so percent of her professional time with direct patient care, including diagnosis and follow-up visits; and we learn about her involvement in a new pavilion at Jefferson dedicated to specialty programs, where she included “my population” as design consultants.

Following the enumeration of Dr. Ross’s credentials and accomplishments, there’s a discussion of anxiety in autistic individuals, of how it stems from communication barriers and heightened sensory awareness, of how it can affect performance, and of why environment is so important for neurodiverse populations.

Reimann then offers Dr. Ross’s CV as an exhibit, and Dr. Ross as an expert.]

Reimann: So plaintiffs offer Dr. Ross as an expert in behavior and developmental pediatrics and the diagnosis and treatment of autism.

[At this point, Mike Kristofko, the lawyer for the Lower Merion School District, interjects the following:]

Kristofco: I don't have any basis to question her qualifications in those areas. The motion was really about her opinion she was given about communication, which she is not being offered as an expert in.

The Judge: Okay. So for purposes of this hearing, we will certainly have her as an expert in that.

[Reimann agrees, and her examination resumes.]

Reimann: Can you tell us just -- how does autism affect a person's ability to communicate?

Ross: So, communication has a few parts. There's receptive language, our understanding of language; our expressive language, our use of language or communication; and then there is the motor component of expressive language, which is sort of like the sub-area. And when we think about language, it's not just about spoken language, there is nonverbal communication. Somehow we use our eyes, our hands to communicate, how we point, socially reference.

[Here we see Dr. Ross, like other FC believers, making much of the motor component of expressive language, as if motor difficulties are the main challenges in non-speaking autism, which they aren’t.]

Reimann: Okay. And how does the inability to reliably communicate affect persons with autism?

Ross: Well, if you can't communicate, you could get easily frustrated or anxious.

Reimann: And can autism also affect motor movement?

Ross: Individuals with autism might have a low tone or they might have a component of apraxia, which is a problem with motor planning. It can affect different parts of your body. It can affect, you know, like your overall ability to move. It could affect all different parts.

[Many individuals with autism have low muscle tone, and some have apraxia of speech, but there’s no evidence that individuals with autism are impaired in their “overall ability to move.” Most attested motor difficulties in autism are difficulties other than apraxia: see this discussion.]

Reimann: Okay. So I’ve heard of apraxia speech, but apraxia, is it something more than just --

Ross: You could have apraxia of any body part that moves.

[That doesn’t sound quite right. The various proposed non-speech apraxias (and not all are agreed upon; see Wikipedia for a review) are these:

·         nonspeech oral apraxia (smiling, puckering, blowing, or swallowing)

·         limb-kinetic apraxia (which mostly pertains to finger movements, e.g., tying shoes or typing)

·         gait apraxia (difficulty walking)

·         constructional apraxia (e.g., difficulty copying a simple diagram or drawing basic shapes) and

·         oculomotor apraxia (difficulty with voluntary eye movements).]

Reimann: Okay. And... is there an interrelationship between the executive functioning and autism or an intersection there?...

Ross: Yeah. So a lot of individuals with autism have the executive functioning deficits that can impact apraxia. There is an interplay because it affects motor planning, so executive functioning is sort of like our inner administrative executive that helps us execute what we want to do. So it can impact motor movements. So that things that are automatic can be done quite easily, but things that need spontaneous ongoing development can be very complicated.

Reimann: Okay. So just in terms of Alex, are you aware that -- so like that he can type, like, lists of things. Do you know that?

[Court documents report that A.L. can transcribe lists of words by independently typing them out on an electronic keyboard.]

Ross: Yes.

Reimann: And so how is that different than -- from being able to spontaneously generate thoughts through typing?

Ross: So some individuals with autism, instead of a repetitive behavior, they can have -- well, his repetitive behavior is hyperlexic, so they can type repeatedly or have special interests, but that's different than communication. That's just a repetitive sub-stimulatory behavior for him.

[This is the first time I’ve heard anyone propose hyperlexia/typing words from lists as an example of a repetitive behavior in autism. More typical examples are hand flapping, rocking, toe-walking, head banging, and the spinning of or lining up of objects (see here).]

Reimann: And how does executive functioning sort of come into play with that?

Ross: Well, that does not require executive functioning, because it's something that is done by approach.

[While transcribing a list of words by typing them out doesn’t involve as much executive functioning as communicative typing, it does involve motor planning—and thereby rules out apraxia. That is, regardless of how “repetitive and self-stimulatory” and non-communicative A.L.’s typing transcription abilities/activities are, they aren’t consistent with a diagnosis of any sort of non-speech, non-oral apraxia.

Limb-kinetic apraxia, again, mostly pertains to finger movements, e.g., typing.

Constructional apraxia entails difficulty copying things, e.g., words.

And oculomotor apraxia entails difficulty with voluntary eye movements, which includes those required for transcribing a list of words via typing, particularly when one is typing with just a pointer finger (as is reportedly the case with A.L.) as opposed to touch typing. Transcribing a word with a pointer finger would require deliberately moving one’s eyes back and forth between a word on the list and the keyboard.]

 [With no further discussion of apraxia or typing, and no discussion at all of why Alex needs a letterboard to be held up in order to type in a non-repetitive, non-self-stimulatory, communicative way, the examination turns to the heterogeneity of autism. It’s only after this discussion that Reimann turns to letterboard-based communication.]

Reimann: So let's talk about Alex's communication with the letter board. Was Alex your first patient to communicate with a letter board and a communication partner?

Ross: Yes.

Reimann: And have you had other patients after Alex who communicate with a letter board and communication partner?

Ross: I have.

Reimann: And have you had other patients who have tried to communicate with a letter board and communication partner but have been unsuccessful?

Ross: Yes.

[Patients, plural. This is significant.

First, it throws into question the high success rates proclaimed by S2C practitioners, most notably S2C “inventor” Elizabeth Vosseller. One of Vosseller’s most widely broadcast statements is the one that opens the pro-S2C documentary Spellers: “There’s never any doubt in my mind when someone walks into my room that they can and will spell for me.”

Second, it raises the possibility that some families who briefly try out S2C quietly disappear and keep their mouths shut—except in private conversations with, say, their pediatricians.

Third, it raises the question of what S2C failure looks like. Is it lack of compliance by the autistic individual? Perhaps not all minimal speakers with autism are willing to “tolerate” S2C—i.e., to sit or stand still and poke at a letterboard held in front of their faces, at least for long enough to spell apparently meaningful phrases.

Or is S2C failure the result of a decision by the parents to stop using S2C? Perhaps for some parents, red flags appear early on. Perhaps they discover (as some report they have) that they never have any success when they’re the ones facilitating their child and find this troubling enough to abandon S2C (not everyone does). Or perhaps they find themselves confronted, when the S2C practitioner first “unlocks” their child, with S2Ced output exhibiting literacy and verbal sophistication at skill levels that just don’t seem like something their child could have secretly attained (I know of at least one parent who abandoned S2C for this reason).

As we’ll see below, however, Dr. Ross has something completely different in mind for S2C failure.]

[At this point in the proceedings, the judge establishes that A.L. has been Dr. Ross’s patient for 8-10 years, and there’s some discussion about where Dr. Ross was affiliated when she first started seeing him.]

Reimann: Okay. And for the patients who have tried communicating with a letter board and communication partner, for the ones that have been unsuccessful, did they continue to use the letter board and communication partner even though they were not having success?

Ross: No.

Reimann: And approximately how many times have you been with Alex when he has communicated with a letter board and communication partner?

Ross: I don't know an exact number of times, but he has done it in my office; he has done it at experiences we have had in the community. He presented at a couple of conferences. One was a symposium that we have, which is an international symposium on autism and the built environment. And he and some of the other individuals who spell to communicate presented it.

[There is a clarification that this was during COVID and that the presentations were on Zoom.]

Ross: So... part of the statements were prewritten and then people did ask questions. And spellers communicated on Zoom. And...  the way we had them set up is people could see them spelling on the camera. It wasn't just Alex, it was also some other individuals.

[Here the judge jumps in.]

The Judge: I guess I have seen some of the videos, so I am trying to understand, because it seems to me that when he is using the letter board -- and I’ve only seen a very minor segment of videos, not a huge amount. But how did that work when he was asked questions in terms of -- it seems to me like he touches the letter board, touches the letter. The communication partner may or may not say the letter out loud. They might write it down. But it sort of seems like it's a run-on sentence. Does that make sense?

Ross: Sometimes, Yes. He has some tactical errors.

[“Tactical errors.” I’ve never heard this term used before in the context of S2C, and I have no idea what Dr. Ross meant by it.]

The Judge: Forget the errors. Just say that if he does a whole running sentence, once you see it at the end, you can see the sentence. But say he wrote, “I am happy and sad.” He might do that whole thing. And then once you see it, okay, the letters all make out that sentence, right?

Ross: I see what you are saying. But I guess from what I remember from the conferences, his statements were much shorter.

The Judge: Like “yes” or “no” or something?

Ross: Or more like short answers, not complete sentences.

The Judge: So it's obvious what the word is then once he spells? Everybody in the audience knows?

Ross: Yes.

The Judge: And nobody has to say it out loud?

Ross: No.

The Judge: So you see it based on him doing the letters?

Ross: Touching the board, yes.

[A couple of clarifying questions ensue.]

The Judge: Do you remember an example of what he said?

Ross: I mean, I think it varies. Sometimes when he writes, like it's pretty obvious, like you hear the letters and you know. Sometimes I have had it where if it's just he and I in my office and his statements tend to be longer, sometimes I will write it and then I will look at it....

[This statement appears to suggest that Alex has typed messages in Dr. Ross’s office without a communication partner present—unless Dr. Ross herself served as his communication partner.]

[A discussion ensues about how the length of A.L.’s utterances can vary, as is the case for everyone.]  

The Judge: I am trying to understand for your audience how they were able to understand [what A.L. was typing.]

Ross: Right. So they would see him touching the letters, and then sometimes somebody would say it afterwards. But you could see that he used those letters.

[There’s one more clarifying exchange and then Reimann resumes questioning.]

Reimann: For that symposium, who acted as his communication partner?

Ross: It was Tom.

Reimann: Tom Foti?

[Tom Foti is the communication partner who is to assist A.L. at the trial, and who is to be examined and cross-examined later this day.]

Ross: Yes.

[Another clarifying exchange.]

Reimann: And has Alex initiated conversations with you?

Ross: Yes.

Reimann: And do you have an opinion to a reasonable degree of professional certainty as to whether the letter board and communication partner is an effective method of communication for Alex?

Ross: I believe for Alex it is.

Reimann: And do you have any doubt that Alex's communication with the letter board and communication partner are his thoughts and no one else's?

Ross: Yes, I believe that.

Reimann: Do you have an opinion to a reasonable degree of professional medical certainty as to whether Alex's need for a letter board -- as to whether Alex needs a letter board and communication partner to communicate?

Ross: Yes, I think he does.

Reimann: And what is the basis of your opinion? So... you saw Alex before he started communicating with the letter board, correct?

Ross: Yes.

Reimann: And can you just tell us what that communication looked like?

Ross: It was a lot less than when he had the letter board. And I have been with him. For example, when he went to see Dr. Stevens [the doctor who became his new doctor when he transitioned from pediatrics to adult care], I accompanied him, and he told her things that there was no way we would have known. Like he said his ears hurt, and it wasn't like he was touching his ears and she looked in his ears and he had an ear infection. So, you know, I believed that he was communicating. I don't see how just from having watched him, nobody touches him, nobody is moving the board, I don't see how it could not be his communication.

[We’ll return to the ear infection later. For now let’s just note that we see here the lay person’s lack of familiarity with subtle cues (both subtle facilitator cues and subtle pain cues) and the lay person’s susceptibility to naïve realism: the fallacy that we can trust that what we perceive through our senses is a thorough and accurate depiction of reality.]

Reimann: Okay. And do you have an opinion to a reasonable degree of medical certainty as to whether not letting Alex using the letter board and communication partner silences him?

Ross: I think it absolutely silences him.

Reimann: Do you have an opinion to a reasonable degree of medical certainty as to whether communication with the letter board and communication partner is effective for all persons with autism?

Ross: I don't think it's effective for all persons with autism.

[I suspect that the purpose of harping on this point is to lend credibility to Dr. Ross’s judgment that S2C works for A.L. That is, repeated reminders that she’s noticed cases where S2C doesn’t work might help suggest that Dr. Ross is able to discern the difference between S2C successes and S2C failures. In fact, of course, there is no evidence that S2C has ever been successful.]

Reimann: And why is that?

Ross: I don't know. I don't know all the persons with autism. But I do feel like it's effective for Alex.

The Judge: For the patients that you have had that it's been unsuccessful for, do you know why?

Ross: I don't know why. I think sometimes the parents were moving their fingers, like were holding them or touching them. So it wasn't clear to me that it was their communication as opposed to their parents moving their hand. They didn't – you just -- they didn't seem able to communicate things that other people would not have known unless they communicated it like Alex has in the past. And their behaviors didn't seem related to the communication... So they didn't seem to have a change in their behavior and demeanor after using the board. But I don't know why it doesn't work for some people and why it works for other people. But that's no different why I don't know certain other things work for individuals with autism.

[“It wasn't clear to me that it was their communication as opposed to their parents moving their hand”—this disclosure is one of Dr. Ross’s most significant. What she has in mind by S2C failure, as it turns out, isn’t kids who don’t comply or parents who abandon it, but facilitator control. This makes Dr. Ross the only S2C believer I’ve ever heard publicly acknowledge that in some S2C partnerships —ones that she’s actually observed first hand involving her own patients, presumably patients from the general Philadelphia area--the S2C-generated messages may be being controlled by the facilitator.

And yet, in assuming that this happens only to some individuals, Dr. Ross doesn’t explain why it wouldn’t happen to others.

Presumably the point of this line of questioning is to suggest that Dr. Ross is aware of the possibility of facilitator control and, thus, to add credibility to her claim that she has observed no facilitator influence over A.L. Of course, the fact that someone is able to detect the most obvious signs of facilitator control doesn’t entail that they’re able to detect all signs of facilitator control.]

The Judge: And how did Alex communicate with you before the letter board?

Ross: I mean, not a lot. I mean, I don't know. Sometimes -- you know, it's really hard to know when somebody with autism is even engaged in nonverbal communication because sometimes it's easier for them to attend when they are not making eye contact, or they might be having repetitive behaviors. But when he is using the letter board, he can do that for a prolonged period of time. I will ask a question, and he can provide a somewhat complex answer that only he would know. It's reciprocal...

[No specific example is offered here of A.L. outputting a complex answer that only he would know.]

Ross: [Continuing] When we look at autism and we are doing an assessment, we use something called the autism diagnostic observation schedule, and there are different levels of communication. So there is like: can somebody answer a question? Can they answer a question and then elaborate on their answers spontaneously? Can they answer a question, elaborate on their own response spontaneously, and then ask the examiner a question? Because a conversation is very reciprocal. It's like a tennis match. It's not like squash where it's hitting against the wall or you are just serving, right. It's an ongoing, reciprocal, social experience. So the nature of his communication became more reciprocal and sustained by Alex.

[Squash is as ongoing, reciprocal, and social as tennis, actually; apparently the sports venues Dr. Ross has consulted on don’t include the large number of squash facilities in the Philadelphia area, including the world’s biggest.

But more important than Dr. Ross’s squash remarks is her commentary about the autism diagnostic observation schedule (the ADOS). The ADOS is considered the gold standard for measuring autism severity. Dr. Ross makes it sound like A.L.’s scores on the ADOS’s measures of conversational reciprocity would improve dramatically were he measured based on S2C-generated output. And apparently this doesn’t raise any questions in Dr. Ross’s mind about whether that output is authentically A.L.’s.]

[At this point Reimann resumes her questioning with a discussion of A.L.’s transition from her practice to Dr. Stevens practice when he aged out of pediatric care, and this is where we return to the earlier mentioned ear infection.]

Ross: This was when Alex transitioned, and he also had an ear infection and sinus infection.

Reimann: And who was his communication partner? Did he use the letter board to communicate with Dr. Stevens?

Ross: Yes.

Reimann: And who was his communication partner?

Ross: His mother.

[This raises the possibility that his mother was also present and assisting Alex’s letter selection during the previously mentioned visits to Dr. Ross’s office.]

Reimann: And did Alex communicate anything about how he was feeling at that visit?

Ross: Yeah, he said that his ears were hurting and that he had sort of pressure.

Reimann: Okay. This medical record, does that reflect Dr. Stevens's examination of Alex?

Ross: Yes.

Reimann: And what did she conclude?

Ross: She concluded that he had a bilateral otitis media, which is an ear infection. And she also concluded here that she was conversing with him.

[And with that, we’ve arrived at one of the plaintiff’s main exhibits in defense of S2C: the fact that, when A.L. informed Dr. Stevens through S2C that he had an ear infection, her examination of him confirmed this and led her to conclude that A.L.’s S2C-generated message was coming from him, not from his mother.]

Reimann: Okay. And so is this record made as part of a regular medical visit at Jefferson?

Ross: Yeah, this is part of the electronic medical record.

Reimann: And are these records made at or near the time of the visit?

Ross: Yes.

Reimann: And is this medical record created as part of Jefferson's internal record keeping?

Ross: Yes.

Reimann: That's all I have. Thank you.

The ear infection story is the only story I’ve encountered of an S2Ced person communicating information that the communication partner purportedly didn’t have access to. I first came across it in 2021, in a piece by A.L.’s mother entitled “A Letter to My Son’s Skeptics.” In it, A.L.’s mother gives four reasons why A.L. shouldn’t have to participate in a message-passing test—the “most important” being that “he doesn’t want to, and we respect his choice.” That is her fourth reason; her first is that there are “multiple examples” of A.L. communicating information that she says couldn’t have been known to others and that were verified by third parties. She cites only one example, however: the ear infection story.

And that singular story makes a second appearance in court, in the cross-examination of Dr. Ross by the school district lawyers, to which I’ll turn in my next post.


Friday, May 2, 2025

Spelling to Communicate goes to trial

After three months of talking about telepathy, I’m changing the subject to courtroom drama. For the last few years I served as an expert witness in a lawsuit again a school district in suburban Philadelphia. The case, which dates back to 2019, came to an end in January.  While not nearly as extraordinary as telepathy, some of the drama was as odd as Oz.

As in the Land of Oz.

First, some background

Regular readers might recall this case from some earlier posts (here, here, and here). The school district in question, the Lower Merion School District of Lower Merion County, Pennsylvania, made the fateful decision not to hire a trained Spelling to Communicate (S2C) “communication partner” for a non-speaking autistic student (A.L.). The plaintiffs, A.L.’s parents, claimed that this decision violated Section 504 of the Rehabilitation Act of 1973 (“Section 504”) and the Americans with Disabilities Act.

The first ruling in the case occurred at the conclusion of a due process hearing back in December, 2019: the hearing officer ruled against the parents. The parents subsequently appealed to the United States District Court for the Eastern District of Pennsylvania, and in September, 2022, the judge, Judge Marston, denied their appeal.  Weighing heavily in both rulings was the lack of evidence for S2C and the observations of S2C reported by School District staff. As Judge Marston wrote in her ruling:

The fact that S2C is not research based and ASHA’s position statement certainly support the District’s decision not to implement S2C, but the most compelling pieces of evidence are the District’s personnel’s first-hand observations of A.L. using S2C... Ms. Grimley, the District’s speech-language pathologist, observed A.L. using S2C in December 2017. (ODR0105.) She testified that A.L.’s communication partner “us[ed] prompts” and would say things like “closer, go get it, left, up, down, while he was poking the letters.” (Id.) Ms. Van Horn, A.L.’s reading teacher, had a similar experience when she observed A.L. using S2C. (ODR0090.) She noted that he was “flubbled” and unable to provide the correct answers. (Id.) Yet, when his communication partner received the answer key, A.L. started answering the questions correctly, which troubled Ms. Van Horn and suggested to her that A.L.’s communication partner was guiding him to the correct answer. (Id.).

The parents appealed next to the United States Court of Appeals for the Third Circuit, and in June, 2024, the tide started turning in their favor. The Circuit Court ruled that the District Court had wrongfully denied the appeal, sending the case back to the District Court for a summary judgment or a trial by jury. Judge Marston agreed to the latter. A jury trial, of course, shifts the powers of adjudication from hearing officers and judges to... members of the general public. Raising the stakes even further, the plaintiffs decided to put A.L. on the stand, with his testimony facilitated out by an S2C communication partner.

Given how convincing S2C in action appears to be to be to most people—at least judging from the large number of positive news stories about S2C and the positive comments that predominate in public videos of even the least convincing cases of FC/RPM/S2C—things were no longer looking so good for the School District. Especially since it was unclear whether they could convince Judge Marston to allow an examination, via well-controlled message-passing tests, of who was authoring the S2C-generated messages—A.L., or his communication partner.

So the School District offered the plaintiffs a settlement—which the plaintiffs turned down. The parents were, perhaps, not only confident of winning, but eager to set a legal precedent. A decision in their favor would enable all the other families in the district who wanted the Lower Merion schools to approve and fund S2C communication partners for their minimally speaking autistic children as well—and perhaps set a precedent for similar parents all around the country.



Down the rabbit hole—or over the rainbow

On November 6th, 2024, the trial was set: January 7th, 2025. A tornado of legal activities was set in motion.

The school district had two expert witnesses for this case: Dr. Howard Shane (who testified in the original hearing) and myself (brought on after the appeal). As the more local of the experts (Howard Shane is based in Massachusetts), I was asked to attend a special hearing at the federal courthouse of the Eastern District of Pennsylvania—a ten minute train ride from home. The hearing was Daubert hearing, aka a “voir dire” (an oral questioning/examination) of several of the plaintiff’s expert witnesses whom the School District sought to disqualify. The plaintiffs had lined up some impressive experts, first and foremost Barry Prizant, Ph.D., CCC-SLP, well-known for his early work on echolalia and for his recent support for S2C. Prizant, however, wasn’t among the School District’s voir dire targets; these instead included a neuropsychologist and an S2C communication partner. The neuropsychologist was Anne Robbins, whom the parents had hired to do an independent, neurocognitive assessment of A.L. that was based, in part, on output that A.L. generated via S2C. The S2C communication partner was Tom Foti, whom the plaintiffs had designated as A.L.’s communication partner for the upcoming trial. It was the voir dire of Tom Foti that the school district wanted me to attend.

But when I showed up at the designated time and place, the earlier voir dire, that of neuropsychologist Anne Robbins, was still underway. She was being asked questions by Nicole Reimann, one of the plaintiff’s lawyers (Ms. Reimann being the only of those attorneys in attendance at this hearing). Dr. Robbins would then be handed off to the School District lawyers (of which there were three, all in attendance, but only one of whom, Mike Kristofco, would be asking questions).

What follows here and in my next few posts is based on a publicly available document: publicly available but heftily pay-walled and requiring some dozen steps for access. This document is a transcript of what happened on Monday December 2nd, 2024, in courtroom 15b in the Federal Courthouse on 6th and Market Street in downtown Philadelphia. I’ve converted it to more user friendly format (it was originally in all caps), and annotated it here and there in bracketed italics with additional impressions based both on my notes (21 pages of hasty scrawls) and on some additional post hoc reflections. I’ve also omitted those parts of the transcript that are more procedural in nature and/or otherwise less relevant to the key question for us here at FacilitatedCommunication.org: What can we learn about the validity of the instances of Spelling to Communicate that have arisen in this case?

In this first installment, I present excerpts and impressions from the first half of the voir dire of Dr. Robbins, where she is being questioned (in a “direct examination”) by Ms. Reimann, the plaintiff’s lawyer. A.L., throughout these proceedings, is referred to as “Alex.”

Reimann: Good afternoon, Dr. Robbins. How do you know Alex le Pape?

Robbins: I completed two psychological evaluations with Alex.

Reimann: And was the first one in 2018; is that right?

Robbins: 2018 was the first one; 2021 was the second.

Reimann: And when you say evaluate, what kind of evaluation did you do; can you tell us a little bit more about that?

Robbins: I was assessing certain aspects of cognitive ability and some aspects of academic abilities. And the second evaluation there was a little bit of memory testing that was done, and an adaptive behavior skills was completed.

Reimann: And do you hold any professional licenses?

Robbins: I am licensed as a psychologist in Pennsylvania.

Reimann: And do you hold a certification in Pennsylvania, as well?

Robbins: I am certified as a school psychologist in Pennsylvania.

[What follows here is further discussion of Robbins’ professional qualifications, including her doctoral degree from Widner University and her post-doctoral training at Nemours AI Dupont Children’s Hospital; her work at the now-closed Child Study Institute and Bryn Mawr college; and her work in private practice doing neuropsychological evaluations and consulting with parents and schools, in which capacity, she reports, she has evaluated “somewhere between 750 and 800 students,” of which “close to 50 percent” have a diagnosis of autism. After this, the examination turns to A.L. and a cognitive assessment Robbins did of him in 2021 using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales, 4th edition (WAIS-IV). Ms. Reimann begins by asking Dr. Robbins to read out one of the test’s guidelines.]

Reimann: If you could read the first three sentences of the block quote, and if you could just read those out loud, please.

Robbins: Quote, examinees with special needs, such as physical, language or sensory limitations, are frequently referred for psychological evaluation. With such examinees, it is important not to attribute low performance on a cognitive test to low intellectual ability when, in fact, it may be related to physical language or sensory difficulties.

Reimann: Okay. And have you seen -- you personally seen instances in your practice where scores underestimate the intellectual ability if the test is administered in a standard fashion?

Robbins: Yes.

[There is then a short discussion of modifications made during testing, including allowing the presence of a behavioral specialist and what roll they’re allowed to play. Then the questions turn to modifications made for A.L.]

Reimann: Can you tell us what modifications you made when you tested Alex?

Robbins: He used an alternative form of communication, the Spelling to Communicate method, because he is nonverbal, but he is essentially verbal with that methodology. And that methodology requires the presence of a communication partner. So there was a communication partner in the room at that time, which was his mother. So those were really the two adaptations.

Reimann: Did you give his mother instructions on what she could or could not do within -- during that -- in her role as communication partner?

Robbins: Yes.

Reimann: And what did -- what were those instructions?

Robbins: Just that she was not able to explain or, you know, encourage Alex in any way. She was repeating the letters that he was spelling. I told her she could praise him for working hard, but she was not to explain or give any real feedback to Alex while she was being a communication partner.

Reimann: And did Mrs. Binder-le Pape [Alex’s mother], did she comply with those directions?

Robbins: Yes.

Reimann: And so how -- let's just talk about how it is that you came to evaluate Alex. How were you -- how was that brought up as a possibility?

Robbins: So I was contacted by Alex's speech and language therapist who had worked with him for many, many, many years. Her name is Susan Chaplick. She was introduced to the Spelling to Communicate method that the parents and Alex showed that to her as part of her treatment. She made a decision to explore how he would perform given that mode of communication relative to how he performed in the absence of it, and the scores that were generated were significantly better. And so then I guess she had the thought that maybe we should -- maybe the parents should consider exploring what we could learn about his cognitive abilities if he was using this preferred mode of communication. And Susan Chaplick suggested they be in touch with me. And I got a call and a conversation started.

Reimann: Okay. And did you do anything to familiarize yourself with S2C and communication with the letter board and communication partner for Alex?

Robbins: Yes. Well, obviously I talked to Susan Chaplick and I got information from her based on her experience. I reviewed some videos that had been taken of him communicating using the method. And I went to observe him in one of his Spelling to Communicate training sessions at the practice in Springfield where he was undergoing training in the method.

[This clinic is called Inside Voice—it is the S2C provider closest to Philadelphia and Lower Merion County].

Reimann: Can you tell us about that observation in Springfield?

Robbins: So Tom -- Alex was working with a communication partner by the name of Tom Foti. I think that's how you pronounce it. And I was in the back of the room. And I think they were going through a pretty structured and prescribed sequence of tasks. It started with what they called a check-in where Tom asked Alex how he was doing and Alex used the spelling board to say he was happy and he was excited to do the work, et cetera. Then Tom read a passage to Alex, and then there was a prescribed set of questions that were asked of Alex following the passage. And the questions seemed to go from, you know, requiring the least amount of information or response from Alex to more information. So there was questions that had just a one-answer response. And he is spelling all of his responses. And some that requires sort of a sentence length and some interpretation of what happened in the video and what he saw, what it meant. And then at the end of that, I think Tom -- Tom asked him if he wanted to see a video of the -- the passage was about a country music singer. And he asked if he wanted to see a video of the guy. And Alex said, yes. And so they watched the video. And then after watching the video, Tom asked Alex about his impressions. And Alex communicated quite a bit of material about what he saw, what he thought, what his initial impression was, how his impression changed over the course of the video, and he spoke about his admiration for the performer. So that's what occurred in session, that's what I observed.

[Here’s where I entered the room and started taking notes.]

Reimann: And so after you did these things, spoke to Susan Chaplick, looked at the videos and then observed Alex, did you agree to do an evaluation of him with him using the letter board and communication partner?

Robbins: I did.

Reimann: And does Alex use speech to functionally communicate?

Robbins: I did not observe that.

[There follows a brief discussion of what’s meant by functional communication].

Reimann: Okay. And just generally tell the court, what did you learn from the testing you did of Alex? What you did learn about Alex?

Robbins: So his performance on the test certainly in terms of the verbal cognitive abilities were well above average compared to other students his age, obviously using the preferred and effective communication method, his sole method of communicating... His reading comprehension was also well above age and grade expectancy... There was a measure of listening comprehension where he demonstrated average or above capabilities... I attempted a few of the visual, nonverbal tests. And some he was able to complete; some I was not convinced that he understood what I was asking him to do, and I stopped him midway...

[In other words, A.L. has average or above listening comprehension capabilities but somehow didn’t understand what she was asking him to do.]

Reimann: And did you learn anything about his ability to think and communicate in abstract terms?

Robbins: Yes. Because, you know, one of the tests sort of requires some abstract and conceptual reasoning in order to see similarities amongst objects or concepts. The reading comprehension test has questions that require some informational reasoning where he had to put together information and generate what his understanding of what was being expressed in the text.

Reimann: Okay. And did you -- did Alex demonstrate a sense of humor in the time that he was with you?

Robbins: Yes, he did.

Reimann: And can you give us an example of that?

Robbins: I guess the first one that comes to mind is one of the words... on the definitions test... is “reluctant.” And his initial comment was, “Like me today.”

[More discussion of the details of the testing and how Robbins wasn’t able to calculate a full-scale IQ score because not enough subtests were completed.]

Reimann: And for the matrix reasoning, can you describe what that -- how that test is done?

Robbins: That's a visual, nonverbal test that sort of calls on pattern reasoning. So he would have visual stimuli. There would be four quadrants of information. There would be visual stimuli in three of them; there would be an empty quadrant. There would be five choices at the bottom of the manual... And the test taker is supposed to choose which of the options would complete -- would best fit with the pattern of information... They would typically point, or they do have the option to say the number. The options at the bottom have numbers.


 Reimann: Okay. And did you start -- did you initially try to do the test with Alex pointing?

Robbins: Yes, I did.

Reimann: And was he successful with that?

Robbins: No, it was very random. I could not interpret what he was pointing at. He was moving his hands.

Reimann: After that, did you also try to have him use the letter board to point to--

Robbins: Right, the letter board --

Reimann: Tell us what that looked like.

Robbins: So the letter board had number options on it, I think it was on the flip side of the board. So, you know, I proposed, why don't you make your decision and show me by... pointing at the number on your letter board and we will use that as your response.

Reimann: And was he able to do that?

Robbins: Yeah, he was able to do that.

[No question is raised about why A.L. can’t point to a numbered choice on a stationary surface, but can point to a number on a held-up letterboard.]

Reimann: And so ...  you administered that test both in 2018 and 2021; is that right?

Robbins: Yes.

Reimann: And so in 2018 when he was using the letter board, he was at the 16th percentile; is that right?

Robbins: Yes.

Reimann: And in 2021 with that same deviation from standardization, he was in the 5th percentile?

Robbins: Correct.

Reimann: Do you have any hypothesis about – I mean, is that statistically meaningful, that change, if you know, and do you have any hypothesis about why that might have occurred?

Robbins: It's difficult to know. We didn't really talk about this, but his overall presentation was different in the second set of testing. So I think he was more anxious around that testing. Obviously he still did well on the verbal. He was comfortable with the approach we were using.

[Robbins is seemingly unaware that this is a striking reversal of what’s generally found in autism: nonverbal visual pattern recognition scores are significantly higher, not significantly lower, than verbal scores (see, for example, Grondhuis et al., 2018). When it comes to matrix reasoning in particular, a study alluded to by S2C proponent Vikram Jaswal in a recent podcast on Barry Prizant’s Uniquely Human Podcast, Courchesne et al., 2015, finds relatively high performance on this kind of cognitive test as compared to others. Yet somehow, even with the letterboard, A.L. has scored much worse on matrix reasoning than on verbal ability.]

After a few more exchanges about the details of testing, the voir dire of Dr. Robbins is interrupted by the arrival of three people I recognize: A.L., his mother, and Tom Foti, the communication partner who is to assist him at the trial. Robbins steps down for now (she will later be cross-examined by the school district’s team) and Foti takes the stand. In my next post, I’ll discuss what happened next.

REFERENCES:

Grondhuis, S. N.,  Lecavalier, L. L.,  Arnold, E., Handen, B. L., Scahill, L.,  McDougle, C., L., and Aman, M. G. (2018). Differences in verbal and nonverbal IQ test scores in children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 49, 47-55.

Courchesne, V., Meilleur, A. A., Poulin-Lord, M. P., Dawson, M., & Soulières, I. (2015). Autistic children at risk of being underestimated: school-based pilot study of a strength-informed assessment. Molecular autism6, 12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-015-0006-3