Wednesday, June 11, 2025

Spelling to Communicate Goes on Trial: Part IV

This is the fourth part in a series on a Spelling to Communicate (S2C) lawsuit against a school district. You can read more about the background in my first post, but, in brief, the lawsuit arose because the school district, the Lower Merion School District of Lower Merion PA, refused to hire an S2C “communication partner” for the parents’ non-speaking autistic son (A.L.).

This series focuses, specifically, on a hearing that occurred on December 3rd of last year that I now have permission to write about. The hearing was a Daubert hearing, aka a “voir dire” (an oral questioning/examination) of several of the plaintiff’s expert witnesses whom the School District sought to disqualify. These include Tom Foti, the designated communication partner for A.L. for A.L.’s testimony at the upcoming trial on January 7th, and the focus of this post.

In my previous three posts (starting here), I described and excerpted the examinations and cross-examinations of two people:

Dr. Anne Robbins, the neuropsychologist whom A.L.’s parents hired to do an independent, neurocognitive assessment of A.L.: an assessment that was based, in part, on output that A.L. generated via S2C

Dr. Wendy Ross, a highly celebrated figure in the Philadelphia autism and neurodiversity scene and A.L.’s former developmental pediatrician, who testified under oath that she believes that S2C works for A.L.

Takeaways from the previous posts include, from Dr. Robbins:

That A.L. has average or above listening comprehension capabilities but (on certain nonverbal subtests) didn’t understand what she was asking him to do.

That A.L. couldn’t point to a numbered choice on a stationary surface but could point to a number on a held-up letterboard.

A.L.’s scores, based on S2C-generated output, were much higher on verbal subtests than on nonverbal visual pattern subtests: a striking reversal of what’s generally found in autism.

And, from Dr. Ross: 

That apraxia is something that affects “any body part that moves” (not really), and that bodily apraxia is associated with autism (not really).

That she’s witnessed evidence of facilitator control over messages in some of her patients: the first public admission of facilitator control that I’m aware of by someone who believes that S2C is valid.

That clinical observation trumps formal testing and that holistic assessments trump scientific measurements, and that observations that “the board is not being moved and no one is touching him” establish that A.L.’s S2C-generated output must represent messages coming from A.L. rather than from his communication partner.

That there’s more cause for concern about falsely accusing someone of not communicating than about the possibility that this person’s communication is being hijacked, and his personhood suppressed, however unwittingly, by someone else.

Now to Tom Foti. Besides serving as A.L.’s communication partner, Mr. Foti is the founder of the Philadelphia area S2C clinic Inside Voice. He founded Inside Voice after his parents started doing S2C with his brother, making it a part of the nonprofit A.A.L.I.V.E. that his parents founded in 2007.

Unlike the examination and cross-examination of Dr. Ross, which took place earlier in the morning, I was present for Mr. Foti’s examination, just as I was for the examination Dr. Robbins—the examination with which I opened this series. The Robbins examination, readers of my first post might recall, was interrupted when Mr. Foti entered the courtroom. Foti’s availability, it seemed, was limited; his examination would now take precedence.

I recognized Mr. Foti—I’d seen images of him on the Inside Voice website and elsewhere in S2C promotional materials. A pleasant, soft-spoken man, he looked to be in his late 20s or early 30s. I also recognized the two people who accompanied him, A.L. and his mother. They, too, were familiar to me from S2C promotional materials. A.L., a full-grown man in his early 20s, wore headphones: a common device for addressing the sound sensitivities associated with profound autism. A.L. and his mother sat down in the front row, their backs to me (I was sitting in the far back of the courtroom). 

As has been the case throughout this hearing, A.L. is referred to in these proceedings as “Alex.” And as before, I’ve put my commentary in bracketed italics and elided the parts of the examination that are more procedural in nature and/or less relevant to the questions that concern us here at FacilitatedCommunication.org. These questions are, as before: what can we learn about the validity of the instances of S2C that have arisen in this case? And, in the case of the cross-examination of Mr. Foti: what sorts of beliefs do we see in a long-term S2C practitioner whose close family member is an S2C client? And what sorts of justifications for these beliefs does someone who is probably a true believer in S2C provide while being cross-examined under oath?

The direct examination, by Nicole Reimann, the plaintiff’s lawyer, is brief. It’s established that Foti has worked with A.L. for about seven years but last serviced him on a regular basis three or four years ago, before COVID. During this examination (I quote here directly from my notes) “we hear Alex say ‘car’ and ‘truck’; mom keeps shushing him.” 

After Reimann concludes her examination, the Judge jumps in with a few remarks and questions for Foti, mostly concerning whether Foti will be reading the letters out loud as A.L. points to them when he takes the stand. I’ll begin with the conclusion of this exchange.

The Judge: You will then be saying the letters out loud.

Foti: Yes. If that would be most helpful for everyone, I can do that.

The Judge: All right. From the videos I have looked at, it does not—it looks like once all of the letters—whatever he is communicating—all of the letters are said, then you sort of take those letters and read the entire sentence? 

Foti: Correct. I stop every three to five words to write them down or say them out loud so I am not trying to remember an entire sentence in my head or multiple sentences in my head. That's the most effective way that I have been able to do it.

[At this point the cross-examination begins. Mike Kristofco, the school district’s lawyer, dives right in.]

Kristofco: Mr. Foti, just so we are clear, you don't have any licenses to do what you do, correct? No state or governmental licenses to do what you do?

Foti: The I-ASC does not have a licensed program in the state in the state of Pennsylvania, no.

[The I-ASC is the organization set up by Elizabeth Vosseller, the “inventor” of S2C, to train practitioners and to promote S2C. I don’t believe it has a licensed program in any state.]

Kristofco: And my understanding is you went through a six-month training program with Elizabeth Vosseller in order to be trained as communication partner?

Foti: Yes, the “I-ASC Cohort” is now what it is officially known as.

Kristofco: Is there a training manual that you used?

Foti: Yes.

Kristofco: And—

Foti: A manual or a syllabus.

Kristofco: Syllabus? 

Foti: Manual, syllabus. We were given materials ahead of time.

Kristofco: Did those written materials include rules for what you were and were not allowed to do as a communication partner?

Foti: From an ethical standpoint, yes.

Kristofco: What does that mean?

Foti: There are—without diving too deep into the history, there is other communication methods that have been controversial in the past. And what Spelling to Communicate has done is learn from the mistakes that were made and integrated structure, integrated ethics to make sure that things were structured and as replicable as possible.

[Foti is referring to classic, touch-based facilitated communication (FC), debunked by a dozen authorship (message-passing) studies in the 1990s, from which proponents of held-up letterboard variants of FC routinely attempt to distance themselves (as we see, for example, in some of the documents in this compilation of letters objecting to the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association’s position statement against RPM/S2C).]

Kristofco: Can you explain what those ethical rules are that you will be following as the communication partner for Alex here in court?

Foti: So from an ethical standpoint, it is my duty, my obligation to Alex to say the letters and the words that he has—that he has pointed to on the letter board, not to change them or alter them in any sort of way. The words and the letters that Alex—the letters that Alex pokes are the letters that I say. And it's also my ethical duty that, in the event that from a body standpoint, if I am seeing that Alex is becoming dysregulated to a point where he might be injurious to himself or potentially injurious to others, it's my obligation and my duty to look out for that and make sure that he—that I can get him back into a more regulated state before any sort of continuation may happen.

Kristofco: That last piece you are talking about, that has nothing to do with him poking letters, that just has to do with you tending to him and you knowing how he reacts to certain situations, correct?

Foti: They walk hand in hand, I would say. I would compare it to if a coach sees that one of his players is very emotional for one reason or another, it's the coach's responsibility to bring that player back into a more regulated and more even-keeled sense so that the player can perform at his or her best ability. That's how—that’s the best layman way that I can explain. 

[Before he became an S2C practitioner, Foti, who graduated from the University of Scranton in 2014, was a sports writer and a hockey coach.]

[The judge now jumps in to clarify.]

The judge: So it might mean take a pause?

Foti: Right.

The Judge: Deep breath?

Foti: Yes, correct.

Kristofco: Can anyone who is trained as a communication partner in Spelling to Communicate work with Alex the same way that you can?

Foti: As a practitioner—it's a bit of a layered question. So anybody who is trained in Spelling to Communicate like I am is considered a practitioner. Anyone who has coached specifically to work with just Alex is considered a communication and regulation partner. And the primary difference as a practitioner, I am coached and I am taught and trained how to work with any non-speaker, limited speaker who walks through the doors at Inside Voice and to be able to coach their bodies and teach them this methodology within the structure of Spelling to Communicate. 

[I hadn’t been aware of this difference between “practitioner” and “communication and regulation partner.” It strikes me that this two-tiered system is a way for S2C clinics to:

1. maximize income, via a “professional” cadre that has to pay for six months of training and that, once “certified,” can charge for its services, and 

2. maximize its appeal to parents. S2C, after all, would be a lot less appealing—i.e., a lot less intimate, affordable, and convenient—if parents had to depend on practitioners to extract all their children’s messages.

]

Foti: I apologize, I forget your initial question.

Kristofco: The question was, can anyone who is trained as a communication partner in Spelling to Communicate work with Alex the same way that you can?

Foti: Yes.

Kristofco: When acting as a communication partner for Alex, do you influence in any way what he says?

Foti: I do not.

Kristofco: Is it necessary for you to understand a question in order for Alex to answer it?

Foti: It's helpful for the two of us to be on the same page.

Kristofco: That's not the question. The question is, is it necessary for you to understand the question in order for Alex to answer it?

Foti: It is important for me to understand the question.

Kristofco: Is it necessary for you to understand the question in order for Alex to answer it?

Foti: Yes.

[Wow. He finally comes right out and admits it. And in doing so, he’s implicitly acknowledging that any message-passing test in which he’s out of the room when A.L. is asked a question is likely to fail.]

Kristofco: And why is that?

Foti: Because Alex may interpret that question differently. If we are on different pages on how we interpret the question, then I can't be an effective communication partner for Alex. Because he might be answering the question one way, and I might be–I might have some confusion on what he might be trying to communicate at that point in time. 

[Why should there be any confusion? Aren’t A.L.’s letter selections enough to show what he’s trying to communicate? If the facilitated person’s letter selections aren’t, by themselves, sufficient for successful S2C-based facilitation, and if there’s something more the facilitator needs to know about what the intended message is, then:

1. There’s even more reason than there already was to suspect that the facilitator, not the facilitated person is controlling the communications.

2. Even in the best-case scenario—where there’s somehow no facilitator control—there are still major limitations on what the facilitated individuals can communicate. That is, they’re limited to stuff that their facilitator already knows they want to communicate.

Foti appears to be unaware that no other sorts of communication partners—whether they’re sign language interpreters, interpreters between two different spoken languages, or assistants in partner-assisted scanning—are constrained in this way. He doesn’t seem to realize, that is, that none of these other sorts of communication partners need to understand the questions or know what the person is trying to communicate, beyond their actual words, actual signs, or actual selections, in order to be effective communication partners.]

Foti: [Continuing] So for me to understand the question, it is necessary for me to be the best communication partner and be the best practitioner for Alex.

[Having first stated that to be an effective communication partner, he needs to understand the question, Foti is now stating that to understand the question, he needs to be an effective communication partner. Depending on whether or not being a communication partner and understanding the question are phenomena that inherently co-occur, this borders on either a tautology or an impossibility. Or maybe Foti, possibly nervous, simply misspoke.]

Kristofco: I think I am a little confused.

[So, apparently, is the judge, who jumps in here.]

The Judge: I don't understand. What do you mean by "understand the question?"

Kristofco: Sure.

The Judge: Maybe we don't understand the question we are supposed to understand.

Kristofco: Let me give you an example. If I were to ask Alex a question about a picture that you have not seen and the question is, “Describe what you see in the picture,” but you have not seen the picture, does that prevent you from being a communication partner for Alex?

Foti: I believe you are referencing something that was done a handful of years ago that didn't have an ethical place in validating this communication method.

[Assuming “something that was done a handful of years ago” refers to those message-passing tests that were done in the 1990s—many of which involved showing the facilitated person pictures that their facilitators didn’t see—it appears that Foti believes that message-passing tests have no “ethical place” in “validating” facilitated communication.

But Kristofco clarifies that that is not what he was actually referencing.]

Kristofco: Actually, it's a question that I just asked you. If I were to show Alex a picture that you didn't see and I said—the question is: “Alex, can you please describe what is happening in that picture.” The fact that you have not seen the picture, does that mean that you cannot act as a communication partner for him?

Foti: From an ethical standpoint, I would not be able to take part in that.

[Even the judge is apparently surprised by this.]

The Judge: Why?

Foti: It's written within the ethics that we—that a practitioner from an ethical standpoint, as a practitioner, I can't take part in those sorts of questions because of the history of what was done in the past. Because it was done in a way that was unethical to the—to the speller and to the communication partner.

[This was the first I’d heard of a written ethics code for S2C practitioners that rules out message-passing tests—“because of what was done in the past.” This suggests that anyone who breaks what once was just a tacit rule against testing now risks losing their S2C “license.”]

The Judge: I guess I am confused. What is unethical about it?

Foti: Because when I first begin with any sort of speller, I tell that speller–the first conversation that I have is that, “I believe you are smart and I believe that you don't have anything to prove to me.” And for me to take part in something like that would be unethical because it would undermine the conversation and the belief that I have in that speller right from the get-go. 

[Foti’s rationale here recalls Dr. Ross’s notion that testing communicates that “I am going to set you up and see if this is real” and “would have eroded the trust.” Indeed, this is yet another version of the “presume competence” mantra that goes back to the earliest days of FC. 

And, as with Dr. Ross, Foti’s rationale involves two other problematic lines of reasoning:

First, there’s the circular (or elliptical?) reasoning in Foti’s worry that a message-passing test would undermine his assurance to A.L. that “I believe you are smart and I believe that you don't have anything to prove to me.” This worry has no basis unless A.L. actually understands the words “believe,” “smart,” and “prove”. But “believe,” “smart,” and “prove“ are sophisticated, abstract words that A.L. is likely to understand only if he also understands the more basic, concrete words required to pass a message-passing test. In other words, conducting a message-passing test could only possibly undermine Foti’s assurance to A.L. if A.L.’s vocabulary is well beyond the level needed to type correct answers in a message-passing test. And yet, Foti has implicitly suggested (see above) that any message-passing test in which he’s out of the room when A.L. is asked a question is likely to fail.

And, second, there’s the implicit assumption that the possibility of falsely suggesting that .A.L. isn’t able to communicate is worse than the possibility that the A.L.’s communication is being hijacked, and his personhood suppressed, however unwittingly, by Foti himself.]

Foti: [Continuing] Now, as a practitioner, it's kind of similar. I can advise, I can recommend that certain things not be done, but I also can't be everywhere all the time and force that. So if a communication regulation partner decides to do that, that's out of my control.

[Foti appears to be elaborating on the distinction he drew earlier between “practitioners” (like himself) and “communication and regulation partners” (like his client’s parents). And he appears to be saying that he can’t force the latter (i.e., the communication and regulations partners) not to “do that” (i.e., to participate in message-passing tests).]

Kristofco: So a minute ago you told me that it's your ethical obligation to only say the letters that Alex points to, correct?

Foti: Yes.

Kristofco: And you don't influence in any way what letters he points to, correct?

Foti: I do not.

Kristofco: And you don't influence in any way the—what he is saying when he is using the letter board, correct?

Foti: I do not influence him in any way, shape or form.

Kristofco: So I don't understand the ethical problem as to, if he sees a picture that you don't see and the question is, can you explain what is happening in the picture, why, under Spelling to Communicate, you are unable to be the communication partner for him, for Alex, to point to the letters and explain what he sees in the picture?

Foti: Because it goes back to a study that was done a handful of years ago that didn't put either side in a position to succeed. It goes into—the speller was shown one photo, the communication partner was shown a different photo. And because of what you had just described there is similar, from an ethical standpoint, I cannot ethically take part in a question like that as a practitioner.

[Now it’s “a” study. In fact, there were dozens.

It strikes me that Foti has been so conditioned by his training and his milieu to think that facilitator-blinding tests are bad that he has no idea how strange this sounds to outsiders.]

Kristofco: Is it necessary for you to hear the question that Alex answers in order for you to function as a communication partner?

Foti: So if I were to leave the room, Alex would be asked a question; I would come back?

Kristofco: Yes.

Foti: I would consider that a message-passing test, and that is also not something I can take part in as a communication partner. As a practitioner.

[“As a practitioner”—he’s said it twice in this context. Foti seems to be evoking his distinction between a practitioner and a communication and regulation partner and his suggestion that the former is limited in ways that the latter may not be. Knowing what I know about what occurred after Foti’s cross-examination—stay tuned for next week’s post—I wonder whether Foti and other members of the plaintiff’s team were trying to keep certain possibilities open.]

Kristofco: It sounds to me like Spelling to Communicate has a set of ethics that prevents it from being tested.

[Indeed. That’s one of the hallmarks of a pseudoscience.]

Foti: Spelling to Communicate has taken part in tests. And it's taken part in studies with the Universities of Cambridge, also with the University of Virginia. 

[Here we go again (see Part III in this series): “Cambridge,” “Virginia”—the same institutions alluded to earlier. And, presumably, the same problematic studies: Jaswal et al.’s eye tracking study (critiqued here and here); Jaswal et al.’s subsequent literacy study (critiqued here); and “studies out Cambridge University,” which aren’t actually studies, but rather a research proposal that has yet to go anywhere.]

Foti: [Continuing]. And we are also very, very—we, as in Spelling to Communicate as a whole, we are very very—we are very cautious about what studies we take part in because there are people who have posed as researchers looking to validate this and then things have been turned around, the rules were changed, and things were changed.

[“Posed as researchers looking to validate this”—Foti appears to view researchers as people who set out to validate things, as opposed to people who set out to find out whether or not things are valid. 

Nor does he explain how “things have been turned around” or how “rules were changed” by these people who purportedly were posing as S2C-friendly researchers.]

Foti: [Continuing] And so we very much vet the researchers and the studies who are interested in doing this to make sure that it's done in a way that doesn't ask a speller to try and prove that they are understanding and that they are comprehending.

[Again, as in Dr. Ross’s testimony, there’s the assumption that it’s the speller, rather than the method, that’s being tested. But Kristofco moves on.]

Kristofco: When Alex uses Spelling to Communicate, are you permitted to move the board?

Foti: When the board is in front of him, no, I’m not permitted to move the board. However, if Alex touches two or three letters that might not make sense, for example, q and then d, q, v, a, that would be something where I might pull the board away from him and say—and give him some coaching to look directly at his letters, eyes on the targets, any sort of eye prompt to give him that reminder of here is what you need to do with your body, and I would put the board back in front of him. 

[We know from earlier testimony that A.L. is able to copy lists of words by typing them out independently on a blue-tooth keyboard, without any “coaching” about what to do with his eyes or body. I’m aware of no reports of A.L.’s independent typing generating letters that “might not make sense.” What’s so different about typing self-generated words (as opposed to copied words) on a held-up letterboard? We can only speculate.]

Foti: [Continuing] But once Alex begins to spell, that board stays as still as I can possibly hold it. But it does not—it does not move... left, right, up, center... [I’ve elided some minor interruptions here.]

[It’s a common fallacy among facilitators that they don’t move the board, but as researchers have found, it’s actually impossible not to. In nearly all the videos we’ve seen, the board moves (however slightly) left, right, up, and down, and consistently in the direction of whatever the next letter is that “makes sense.”]

[A couple more minor interruptions.]

Kristofco: Earlier you said it was one of your ethical obligations to say the letters that he touches and not change any of the letters. But now you are saying if he touches letters that don't make sense to you, you can then stop him and redirect him to touch different letters?

Foti: Well, letters that don't make sense when they are strung together.

Kristofco: Don't make sense to whom?

Foti: Well, if there is a string of letters that don't really go together, such as q, v and b or q, v, a. It's different if Alex pokes q, u, o. Those are strings of letters that make sense when they are put together. But q, v, a within the context of a sentence or so, that would not make—that would not make a ton of sense.

[Presumably he means that q-u-o could spell a word, but that q-v-a could not.]

Kristofco: Isn't it true that the entire idea behind Spelling to Communicate is that the disabled individual who is doing the spelling is pointing to the letters that they want to communicate?

Foti: I'm not sure I understand your question.

Kristofco: Sure. The whole premise of it is that when you hold the letter board in front of Alex, that he points to letters that he wants to communicate. That's the premise, isn't it?

Foti: The premise is to presume competence, that Alex is understanding and comprehending everything that is going on around him while also simultaneously understanding that Alex has a form of motor planning called “apraxia” and that he needs coaching to be able to move through that motor planning and that apraxia. When we start, we start with some of the basics [of] Spelling to Communicate and helping him poke to letters on the board reliably and accurately so when we get to open-ended communication, he has built up the skills to be able to get those letters. But every so often if a player makes a mistake in the field of play, that does not mean that player does not know what to do. They just sometimes need the coaching to remind them of here is what you need to do to be successful.

Kristofco: A couple of questions from that. One is: who diagnosed Alex with the apraxia?

Foti: Who diagnosed Alex with apraxia?

Kristofco: Yeah.

[This is a key question; it also came up in the examination of Dr. Ross (see above). Every explanation that I’ve ever encountered for:

1. why a non-to-minimal speaker with autism has sophisticated communication skills that are invisible until they are “unlocked” via S2C,

2. why they need a communication partner to hold up the letterboard for them, and

3. why some or all of the spoken language they produce is at odds with what they type, 

invokes apraxia.

That is, *every single person* whose alleged need for S2C I’ve ever heard discussed is said to have some sort of generalized apraxia/dyspraxia.]

Foti: There is a study out there that shows–from Penn State University that says 66 percent—at least 66 percent of people with autism have a form of apraxia. And we go in with that, that the people that we work with have apraxia. 

[The study, commonly cited by FC proponents, is Tierney et al. (2015), and their number is actually 64%. That 64%, moreover, represents speech and oral motor apraxia, specifically, and only included children with communication delays. Other studies (Shriberg et al., 2011 and Cabral & Fernandez, 2021) find little-to-no correlation between autism and speech apraxia. 

As for other types of apraxia, there is no evidence for significant co-occurrences in autism. And as for A.L. in particular, his ability to copy lists of words by typing them into a Bluetooth keyboard isn’t consistent with a diagnosis of any sort of non-speech, non-oral apraxia. To recap what I wrote earlier in this series, it isn’t consistent with:

Limb-kinetic apraxia, which mostly pertains to finger movements, e.g., typing. 

Constructional apraxia, which entails difficulty copying things, e.g., words. 

Or oculomotor apraxia, which entails difficulty with voluntary eye movements, and which includes those required for transcribing a list of words via typing, particularly when one is typing with just a pointer finger (as is reportedly the case with A.L.) as opposed to touch typing. Transcribing a word with a pointer finger would require deliberately moving one’s eyes back and forth between a word on the list and the keyboard.

And yet, because of this study out of Penn State, “we go with that, that the people we work with have apraxia”.]

Foti: [Continuing] Now, Jen and Fred, Alex's parents, might be able to point out a doctor that had diagnosed Alex with apraxia. I don't have that information on hand.

[As we saw, Dr. Ross, who was A.L.’s developmental pediatrician, alluded to apraxia but never diagnosed Alex as having it.]

Kristofco: So earlier when you said Alex had apraxia, you don't actually know whether that's true?

Foti: I go in with the presumption that he is a non-speaker and he does have apraxia, because I have seen that myself.

[How can Foti be confident that he can “see” apraxia in A.L.? Does he think he’s somehow qualified to diagnose it himself?]

Kristofco: My question is: you don't actually know if that's true, Correct?

Foti: Not definitively, no. I don't have a note from a doctor; I don't have an official diagnosis. But I also see that that’s also medical information that Jen and Fred would need to share with me, but we always operated under that presumption, that Alex has apraxia as an autistic adult.

[It’s curious that Foti apparently has never asked for any information from A.L.’s parents to confirm the diagnosis, despite it being integral to the theory and practice of S2C. Apparently the ”at least 66 percent out of Penn State,” wrong though it is, together with what Foti thinks he can see when observing A.L., is enough not to bother.]

Kristofco: Based on what you said here today, it sounds like as the communication partner who would be with Alex at trial, you will be doing more than just holding the board, correct?

Foti: In terms of coaching his body and being a regulated communication partner for him, yes.

Kristofco: And there is also a reason why—an ethical reason why you need to hear the question, understand the question, and be able to answer the question yourself in order to function as Alex's communication partner, correct?

Foti: Well, that last part, I would not need to be able to answer the question myself because if the question is directed at Alex, then my answer does not have much of a bearing on it.

[So, even though Foti can’t participate in a message-passing test in which he doesn’t see the picture, he apparently doesn’t actually need to be able to answer the question—or so he apparently thinks.]

Kristofco: Do you recall when the school district came out to observe Alex in Inside Voice in January of 2018, Alex was not able to get the correct answers on his history exam until Emily, the communication partner, was provided with the correct answers and then he was able to get the correct answers?

[This de facto message-passing test failure, recounted in the Judge’s ruling that I excerpted in my first post in this series, is one of the most damning pieces of evidence against the validity of S2C as used with A.L. In advising the Lower Merion legal team, I’d recommended that they work a reference to that message-passing failure into Foti’s cross-examination. But at this point Nicole Reimann, the plaintiff’s attorney, jumps in.]

Reimann: I am going to object. This seems to go well beyond the scope.

The Judge: I'm going to allow it for the purpose of this. Go ahead.

Foti: I was not in the room at that time, so I cannot speak to what did or did not happen in that session.

[So it goes. Kristofco moves on.]

Kristofco: Can the letter board be placed on a sturdy easel and you just stand behind the easel or stand next to it, doing all of the other things that you do with respect to regulating his body and everything like that for this methodology to work?

[This was a question that Howard Shane, who also advised the legal team, suggested that Kristofco ask.]

Foti: There is independent typing that is done with a keyboard that is on an easel. However, Alex has not reached that level of skills.

[Here we see a conflation of graduating to a keyboard with graduating to an easel, as if only a keyboard can be put on an easel. Kristofco catches this.]

Kristofco: I'm not talking about a keyboard. I'm talking about the same letter board that he uses when you hold it up. Can it just be set up on a sturdy easel in front of him with you standing right beside it doing the other things that you do for him; can that be used instead of you holding the board?

Foti: It has not been done in Spelling to Communicate before, so for right now, no, I would not be able to do that.

[A stationary letterboard “has not been done before”: in other words, the transition to a keyboard, per S2C protocol, always occurs before the transition to a stationary surface. If the real issue is motor control/apraxia, you’d think that the bigger challenge would be moving from the large letters of letterboards to the much smaller letters of keyboards. But if the real issue is facilitator cueing, then the bigger challenge is moving from a letter array that is held up by the facilitator to one that isn’t. And, indeed, most of those who are subjected to S2C never seem to get to stationary letters.]

Kristofco: Does Alex actually have to touch a letter for it to count?

Foti: Yes.

Kristofco: And when Alex touches between two letters, how do you interpret that?

Foti: I do not interpret it at all. If he pokes between two letters, I say “I need you to definitively poke one of the letters that you mean.”

[I’ve rarely seen this; more often, in ambiguous cases, I’ve seen the facilitator deciding which letter was selected without checking in.]

Kristofco: Tell me about this reset. What are the rules for when you do a reset?

[In S2C land, a “reset” is a term of art for when the facilitator suddenly pulls the letterboard away and then places it back in front of the facilitated person’s index finger.]

Foti: So for a reset, if Alex gives three letters within the context of the question that do not make—that don't really make sense, coming back to that q, v and a, there is a couple of different ways that I could go about it. I could either bring his hand back to his lap. I sometimes call that home base for him. And I will keep that board in front of him, give him a few—give him a few coaching tips, such as “Eyes on that letter,” “Look at that letter until you poke,” things along that line. And then I will bring his hand back up. There is also another way that I describe where he hits q, v, a does not make sense—pardon me, did not make sense in the context of the question that he asked—or that he is answering. And I would pull that board back, back away from him, give him the same—give him the same coaching and then put that board back in front of him and see what happened from there.

[Note that Foti is now saying “does not make sense in the context of the question,” as opposed to “does not make sense in terms of English spelling,” as he suggested earlier. This looser definition of “does not make sense” opens up many more opportunities for facilitator influence.]

Kristofco: Can Alex type on a keyboard?

Foti: I believe he has reached that level of skill, although I have not—I have not used the keyboard with him in quite some time because he has advocated for the laminated board being his preferred method.

[Facilitators may, however subconsciously, not only control their client’s messages, but also sense when the client is able to respond to cues that don’t include board movements—e.g., the more subtle head, torso, and hand movement cues that we’ve discussed elsewhere (e.g., here). As a result, a facilitator who knows that his/her client isn’t ready for the elimination of board movement cues may unwitting facilitate messages out of the client that “advocate for” the held-up letterboard to continue as the client’s “preferred method.”]

Kristofco: So if he can type on a keyboard, then there would be no reason for somebody to have to hold the board other than him just not wanting to type on the keyboard. Is that what you are saying?

Foti: Well, when it comes to a keyboard, Alex is still isolating his pointer finger and touching one letter at a time there. And the keyboard is also designed for using two—for primarily using two hands. So if Alex is going to use a keyboard, he will be using just one hand there.

[So what? The fact the keyboard is designed for two hands doesn’t mean it can’t be used with one hand—and, indeed, A.L. routinely and successfully does just that. Furthermore, in all the videos I’ve seen of S2Ced individuals who’ve “graduated” to keyboards, they’re still typing with the pointer finger of just one hand.] 

Kristofco: So the letter board that Alex currently uses, it's clear, correct? 

[Clear as in transparent.]

Foti: Correct. 

Kristofco: And how is it that—is there a way by which when he is here in the courtroom and the jury is seated next to the witness box, how is it that we will be able to ensure that he is actually—or that the letters you are saying are the actual ones that he is touching? How can we arrange that to keep within the confines of Spelling to Communicate? 

Foti: I would say that when Alex touches a letter, it is obvious that--because you will see the board. When he makes contact with the board, it will move a little bit, but that's just contact has been made and he is bringing his finger right back. If—I mean, I would say in terms of—you will see that the board will move back, but it won't—it won't be very, very obvious. But if you are looking to the side, you will be able to see that.

[A great tip for observers, ironically. Too often we focus on the up-down and right-left movements and miss the less obvious movements forwards and backwards.]

Kristofco: Is it allowable under the Spelling to Communicate methodology for a camera to be placed on the board to see exactly what letters he is touching?

Foti: How do you mean by "on the board?"

Kristofco: Focused on the board. So that it can [be?] sitting behind him but just focused on the board so that we can see exactly what letters he is touching?

Foti: Yes, a camera could be set up.

[I was surprised by Foti’s readiness to make this concession. Most videos of FC/S2C/RPM are controlled, edited, and curated by proponents, who make only the best ones public. The videos that aren’t edited and curated (e.g., those recorded during live interviews and aired by other organizations) are generally far less convincing, even to naïve observers. On the other hand, Foti, as the brother of someone who uses S2C, has struck me throughout this examination as super sincere; a true believer. He may well think that a recording will be as convincing to others as the methodology is to him.]

Kristofco: And when you do training sessions for Alex, sometimes you read him a paragraph and ask questions about it, correct?

Foti: Correct.

Kristofco: That's a procedure he is very familiar with, correct?

Foti: That's correct.

Kristofco: So if I were to read something to him and then ask him questions about it, there is no reason why he should not be able to answer them, correct?

Foti: Answer them with you?

Kristofco: Yes.

Foti: It would depend on what level of skills that you have. If I am starting—

[Foti seems to think that Kristofco is proposing to hold up the letterboard and serve as Alex’s communication partner—an odd misunderstanding that suggests that, in Foti’s mind, holding up the letterboard and asking the questions usually go hand in hand.]

The Judge: No, no, no. You [she’s addressing Foti] would be the communication partner doing the answers; he [she’s referring to Kristofco] would read the paragraph.

Kristofco: Let me start that question over again. I think I might have confused you there. I will restate that. So if Alex were on the stand and you were acting as his communication partner and I asked—and I read him a paragraph and then asked him a question about the paragraph, there is no reason why he should not be able to answer those questions using Spelling to Communicate, correct?

Foti: It would be different for him, that's not a—because Alex is used to hearing the paragraph come from me and there are also key words that are read out loud. So it would be different for him to be able to do that. And I think that would put him in a bit of a difficult spot in doing something that is brand new to him in this sort of setting. If it was something that he was able to practice, then I think—then I have all the faith in the world that he could do it. But to do it right here on the spot would be—would be difficult for him, and I would be concerned that Alex's nervousness or whatever he may be going through at that time would make that a praxis element to him more difficult to work through.

[“Alex is used to hearing the paragraph come from me”; someone else reading to him is “brand new” and puts in him “a difficult spot”; doing it “on the spot” would be “difficult”; it may cause “nervousness”; it may raise “praxis” issues (i.e., apraxia)? This is a whole new dimension of facilitator dependence that I’ve never heard discussed before: a dependence on the facilitator to facilitate not just typed messages (productive language), but also reading comprehension (receptive language). And it raises several new questions:

Is there any aspect of language that the facilitator *doesn’t* facilitate?

Why are “key words that are read out loud” a problem? 

Why would practicing first help? 

What is really being practiced here?

And what is Foti, however subconsciously, actually concerned about here?

]

Kristofco: So let me just follow up on that because this whole trial is going to be—his participation in it will be him sitting on the witness stand and being asked questions from his attorneys and from me or another attorney from my office. So is it your belief that Alex will not be able to answer the questions that we ask him at trial using Spelling to Communicate?

Foti: I believe Alex will be able to answer your questions.

Kristofco: Okay. But you are saying if I read him a paragraph and asked him a question about the paragraph, he could not answer that question?

Foti: I think we are talking about two separate things here, because answering a question is much different than—because he has done that before, he has–he has experience doing that. You reading a paragraph to him changes what he is used to.

[So somehow answering a question is different from responding to a paragraph read out loud? But a question might also contain the kinds of “key words” that Foti has suggested are problematic. What’s the difference between reading a key word out loud and saying it out loud when not reading?

The real issue here, perhaps, is that a paragraph in some document whose contents and context are unfamiliar to Foti might take A.L.’s examination to topics where Foti not only doesn’t know the answer, but can’t (however subconsciously) invent one that sounds plausible.]

Foti: [Continuing] And for a population that is very used to routine and repetitiveness, introducing something new in a high-stake setting is—it can be nerve-wracking. And I would not feel right putting Alex in a situation that he has never done before.

[It occurs to me that there’s a convenient overlap between autistic people’s needs and facilitators’ needs: both benefit from routine and repetitiveness. For autistic people, it’s a well-known preference that appears in the diagnostic criteria. For facilitators, it’s a boon for successful facilitation: routine, repetitive messages are ones that have been practiced over time and so are easier to facilitate than novel ones.

And for true believers who, like Foti, are intimately connected to autism, it’s most likely A.L.’s needs (as an autistic person) for routine and repetitiveness, and not his own overlapping needs (as a facilitator), that make Foti feel wrong about subjecting A.L. to anything novel.]

Kristofco: Just so you understand, I don't know if you have been in a trial or not, but it's very, very typical for a witness on the stand to be shown exhibits and to be asked questions about written documents and exhibits in front of them. It's very typical for an attorney to read part of an e-mail or a letter or a document and then ask the person a question about that. And what I'm trying to understand is, is what you are saying is that you don't think that Alex would be able to perform under those circumstances with Spelling to Communicate in a courtroom that's going to be full of jurors and everybody else?

Ms. Reimann: I'm going to object again. I guess we would have to put in that there was actually e-mails that Alex wrote. What Mr. Kristofco has just described as reading a paragraph–

[Are there emails written by A.L. in which he says he can only understand what’s read to him if it’s read to him by certain familiar people?]

The Judge: I'm going to overrule your objection. He is laying out what could very well be—what happens during trial, whether it's from you on direct examination or him on cross-examination. So we do need to know the answer to this question.

Foti: So to describe how an S2C communication—an S2C session unfolds. So—

The Judge: This is not going to be a session, you understand that.

Foti: Yes.

[Another key distinction that’s easily lost in the fuzzy world of the S2C practitioner: an S2C “session” vs. communicating via S2C in non-clinical settings—as in taking the stand in a courtroom.]

The Judge: So I mean the bottom line is are you going to be able to be his communication partner with whatever question is posed to him which may or may not be a question that you understand or know the answer to? And I understand you say you don't need to know the answer because it's going to be Alex's answer, and that's correct, but I don't know whether you are going to understand the question. It's ultimately up to Alex to understand the question.

Foti: And—

The Judge: And if he does not understand the question, I guess he can say, “I don't understand.”

Foti: Yes, he will say that.

[Hearing Foti so readily go along with this now makes me wonder why he didn’t suggest this easy and obvious solution to begin with. If Foti doesn’t know the answer, he can always (however subconsciously) attribute a lack of understanding of the question to A.L. and then facilitate “I don’t understand” out of A.L.]

The Judge: Maybe we should go to that part of this.

[Presumably the Judge is referring to the examination of A.L., which is the next on the agenda.]

Mr. Kristofco: Just one more follow-up, your honor, just a couple of brief questions.

The Judge: Okay.

Kristofco: Is Inside Voice the primary source of your income?

Foti: Yes, all Inside Voice is.

Kristofco: How much of Inside Voice's business is dependent upon Spelling to Communicate?

Foti: Inside Voice is a Spelling to Communicate program, so we are based on sessions and spellers coming in for sessions.

Kristofco: So that would be 100 percent?

Foti: Yes.

Kristofco: And if Spelling to Communicate were proven not to work, that would have a detrimental effect on your income, correct?

Foti: It would.

[Mr. Foti is a true believer in S2C: having watched him on the stand for all this time, I’m sure of it. But true belief isn’t his only investment in Spelling to Communicate.]

Kristofco: That's the last set of questions I have for Mr. Foti.

The Judge: Anything in response to his questions?

Reimann: No.

Thus concludes the cross-examination of Tom Foti. 

As for our key takeaways:

We hear the usual misrepresentations of the usual studies: the “Cambridge” and “Virginia” studies are suggested to have found evidence for S2C (the former isn’t a study and the latter are fatally flawed); the “Penn State” study is claimed to show that most autistic people have some sort of generalized apraxia that explains why they require “coaching” of their bodies so that they don’t hit “letters that don’t make sense” (even when they’re able, as A.L. is, to type accurately on smaller keyboards when copying word lists).

We learn that having the letterboard on a stationary surface is something that “has never been tried.” Instead, the steps towards what S2C practitioners call “independent typing” (typing on a stationary keyboard with the communication partner within auditory and visual cueing range) are a held-up letterboard followed by a held-up keyboard; NOT a held-up letterboard followed by a stationary letterboard. The former—a held-up letterboard followed by a held-up keyboard—favors facilitator cueing over alleged motor difficulties/apraxia.

We learn about a novel sort of facilitator dependence: i.e., that a client will have trouble understanding something that is read out loud to him by someone other than one of his communication partners.

We learn that there’s a written code of ethics prohibiting S2C “practitioners” from participating in message-passing tests. But this ethics code, apparently, does not rule out a non-practitioner “communication and regulation partners” participating in such tests. Nor does it rule out cameras pointed at the letterboard while a practitioner is facilitating someone who takes the stand in a courtroom.

The next person to take the stand is A.L., facilitated by Foti, but without a camera. Stay tuned for my next post.

________________________________________

REFERENCES:

Cabral, C., & Fernandes, F. (2021). Correlations between autism spectrum disorders and childhood apraxia of speech. European Psychiatry, 64(Suppl 1), S209. https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2021.557

Shriberg, L. D., Paul, R., Black, L. M., & van Santen, J. P. (2011). The hypothesis of apraxia of speech in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 41(4), 405–426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1117-5

Tierney, C., Mayes, S., Lohs, S. R., Black, A., Gisin, E., & Veglia, M. (2015). How Valid Is the Checklist for Autism Spectrum Disorder When a Child Has Apraxia of Speech?. Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP, 36(8), 569–574. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000189

________________________________________


Wednesday, May 28, 2025

Spelling to Communicate Goes on Trial: Part III

This is the third part in a series on a Spelling to Communicate (S2C) lawsuit against a school district. You can read more about the background in my first post, but, in brief, the lawsuit arose because the school district, the Lower Merion School District of Lower Merion PA, refused to hire an S2C “communication partner” for the parents’ non-speaking autistic son (A.L.).

This series focuses, specifically, on a hearing that occurred last December that I now have permission to write about. The hearing was a Daubert hearing, aka a “voir dire” (an oral questioning/examination) of several of the plaintiff’s expert witnesses whom the School District sought to disqualify.

In my previous two posts (here and here), I described and excerpted the “direct” examinations (as opposed to cross-examinations) of two people:

·         Dr. Anne Robbins, the neuropsychologist whom A.L.’s parents hired to do an independent, neurocognitive assessment of A.L.: an assessment that was based, in part, on output that A.L. generated via S2C

·         Dr. Wendy Ross, a highly celebrated figure in the Philadelphia autism and neurodiversity scene and A.L.’s former developmental pediatrician, who testified under oath that she believes that S2C works for A.L.

Takeaways from the previous posts were, from Dr. Robbins:

·         That A.L. has average or above listening comprehension capabilities but (on certain nonverbal subtests) didn’t understand what she was asking him to do.

·         That A.L. couldn’t point to a numbered choice on a stationary surface but could point to a number on a held-up letterboard.

·         A.L.’s scores, based on S2C-generated output, were much higher on verbal subtests than on nonverbal visual pattern subtests: a striking reversal of what’s generally found in autism.

And, from Dr. Ross:

·         That apraxia is something that affects “any body part that moves” (not really), and that bodily apraxia is associated with autism (not really).

·         That A.L.’s autism social reciprocity improves when he is subjected to S2C.

·         That she’s witnessed evidence of facilitator control over messages in some of her patients: the first public admission of facilitator control that I’m aware of by someone who believes that S2C is valid.

·         That at a visit with his new doctor, with his mother holding up the letterboard, A.L. typed out that his ears were hurting, and the doctor subsequently verified that he indeed had an ear infection.  This ear infection story, which has been recounted by A.L.’s mother in a blog post she wrote for a pro-S2C website about why A.L. won’t participate in message-passing (authorship) tests, is the only specific example I’ve heard recounted of an S2Ced person communicating information that the communication partner purportedly didn’t have access to.

I now turn to what happened right after the direct examination of Dr. Ross by one of the plaintiff’s lawyers: the cross-examination of Dr. Ross by one of the school district’s lawyers. The lawyer conducting this examination was Mike Kristofco, and in the context of this Daubert hearing, his objective was to call into question Dr. Ross’s credentials as an expert witness for the plaintiffs.

As is the case throughout this hearing, A.L. is referred to as “Alex.” And as before, I’ve put my commentary in bracketed italics and elided the parts of the examination that are more procedural in nature and/or less relevant to the questions that concern us here at FacilitatedCommunication.org. These questions are, as before: what can we learn about the validity of the instances of S2C that have arisen in this case? And, in the case of the cross-examination of Dr. Ross: what leads an accomplished medical professional who is highly experienced in autism to believe in the validity of S2C? And what sorts of justifications for this belief does this autism professional provide while being cross-examined under oath?

Even in so-called “no touch” forms of FC (like S2C/RPM), facilitators often use visual, auditory, and tactile cues to aid in letter selection. Facilitators may or may not be fully aware of the extent to which these “cues” influence and control letter selection.

Kristofco begins with an allusion to the direct examination of Dr. Ross that had just concluded.

Kristofco: Good morning, Dr. Ross. I want to ask you to clear up a couple of things from the direct examination before I get into my questions. One of the questions that you were asked was about how Alex communicated prior to using the letter board. And I think that your response was something along the lines of “Not as well.” But what I want to ask specifically about is how he actually did communicate before he was using the letter board. So let me ask you: prior to him using the letter board, would he ever answer any questions verbally?

Ross: No.

Kristofco: Did he ever speak at all?

Ross: Yes.

Kristofco: And in what context did he speak?

Ross: It would more be a vocalization. It would not necessarily be in the form of a functional, communicative word.

Kristofco: Were you aware that when he was at school he could ask to go to the bathroom or ask to go to the nurse verbally?

Ross: Yes.

[In other words, A.L. has some functional oral language—language about which there’s little question of authorship, i.e., about who’s doing the talking. That’s because A.L.’s oral language comes straight from A.L.’s mouth rather than from a letterboard held up by someone else. Proponents of S2C, as I discussed in earlier posts (e.g., here), routinely dismiss oral language as unreliable, sometimes calling it merely “reflexive,” and there are reports of S2C practitioners actively discouraging speech.]

Kristofco: Okay. And is he capable of writing anything down?

Ross: He is hyperlexic, so he can write things down, but he usually does not write down a lot of spontaneously put-together things.

[The judge then asks for a clarification of hyperlexia.]

Ross: ... So a lot of kids with autism, they can fixate on letters and words. They can echo them. They can have what is called a stereotypical repetitive behavior with them.

[Ross goes on to discuss the autism diagnostic observation schedule (the ADOS), considered the gold standard for measuring autism severity, and the various aspects of language that make it communicative rather than repetitive and echoic—e.g., joint attention behaviors (though Ross does not use that term). Ross then turns to echolalia.]

Ross: There is people who have echolalia, which is one thing that we score on the test, so that is when they repeat the last word that they have heard. There is something where people can have stereotypical or repetitive phrases, which is something people script, that they repeat phrases that they have heard before but not sort of functionally. And then sometimes it's called gestalt language development when they would script, but they have moved on from scripting non-functionally to using the script to actually get their needs met. So they might answer questions using a script, which, you know, is like a repeated kind of thing, but they have answered in the way it was still a response that was functional for them....

[Ross’s use of the term “gestalt language development” suggests that she may have fallen for a second autism pseudoscience (besides S2C): namely, the theory that non-speaking autistic individuals are “gestalt language processors.” For a systematic review of the lack of evidence for this theory, see here; for commentary on how it undermines evidence-based speech-language therapy, see here.]

[There follows a long discussion by Ross of kids who carry on about preferred topics in a non-social way and how she tries to help make these communications more interactive.]

Kristofco:  And so prior to him using the letter board, the information you needed to treat Alex, did you get that primarily from his parents?

Ross: I would either get it from his parents or from observing him.

Kristofco: Okay. And you had testified about an interaction with—when you were transitioning Alex to Dr. Stevens [Dr. Mary Stevens, the doctor he was transitioned to after aging out of pediatric care] I believe you said his mother was Alex's communication partner and he indicated that he had—through the use of the letter board, that he had some issue with his ears. Do you recall that?

Ross: Yes.

Kristofco: And what was—was Alex—do you recall anything about how Alex was behaving during that meeting?

Ross: I mean, just—he seemed fairly typical Alex to me.

Kristofco: And is it your belief that there is no way that his mother would have known that he had an ear infection?

Ross: I do believe that, because if that had been the case, she would have come in and said, “I think Alex has an ear infection.” She would not have waited for him to generate it. Most parents would do that, if they thought their child was sick and whether or not they could or could not communicate and figure out what they are saying.

[It’s curious that Dr. Ross expects A.L.’s mother to be the one to share any concerning medical symptoms rather than leaving that up to A.L.—who at the time of this appointment was old enough to be aging out of pediatric care.]

Kristofco: Let me show you a document. We can mark this as defendant's exhibit 1. And I have a copy for your honor as well.

[Some logistics ensue pertaining to the passing out of the document.]

Kristofco:  Dr. Ross, this document has been represented to be a declaration that was prepared by Alex. And on the last page of this document it has been purported to be his signature. And what I want to do is just draw your attention to paragraph five, which is on the first page, where it says—the very last sentence, “Sometimes I used to pull my mom's hair when I have an ear infection.” Do you see that?

Ross: Yes.

Kristofco: And what you are saying is that you don't believe that it's possible that Alex was behaving—exhibiting some behavior that his mother picked up on that ended up being communicated through the letter board?

Ross: I do not, because sadly to say, Alex pulled his mom's hair fairly often when he was upset or frustrated. So the fact that he is communicating this does not mean that there weren't other times when he pulled her hair. He pulled her hair, sadly, a lot.

Kristofco: And he had a long history of ear infections as well, correct?

Ross: I mean, he has a history of many things. You know, I was not his general pediatrician, I was his developmental pediatrician. So I didn't treat ear infections in Alex.

[Whether or not Ross treated A.L.’s ear infections, of course, isn’t relevant; what’s relevant is that A.L. had both a history of ear infections and a history of signaling those ear infections to his mother—albeit via a behavior that often occurred for other reasons as well.

And this is a key point. Consider:

·         the association of hair-pulling with ear infections

·         the frequency of those ear infections, and

·         the fact that all parents are acutely sensitive to signs that their children are unwell or experiencing pain in particular parts of their bodies—and that this sensitivity may be largely subconscious and more acute than many people realize.

All this is enough to cast serious doubt on the ear infection story as a convincing instance of A.L. typing out information that his mother-cum-facilitator didn’t have access to.]

Kristofco: You prepared a report in this matter, did you not?

Ross: Yes.

Kristofco: Okay. And I have a copy here.  I will mark this as defendant's exhibit 2. Is that a copy of the report you prepared in connection with this matter?

Ross: I have not seen this in a long time, so I don't—I am just looking at it.

Kristofco: By all means, take a minute to look at it. I realize it has been a number of years since this was created.

Ross: I mean, it looks like—I have not seen it in a long time. I don't know.

Kristofco: Do you recall providing testimony at the due process hearing that was conducted concerning Alex's education?

Ross: Yes.

Kristofco: Okay. And the testimony you provided during that due process hearing was true and correct, correct?

Ross: Yes.

Kristofco: Let me ask you a little bit about your qualifications. You do not have any educational degrees in speech-language pathology, correct?

Ross: No, but I routinely have trained—when I trained at Children's Hospital of Boston, all of our assessments were interdisciplinary. And at Einstein, our assessments were interdisciplinary with the speech-language pathologist. So I am not formally trained in speech and language pathology, but I have worked for a prolonged period of time in interdisciplinary. And that would have been one of the professionals that I worked with. And also, developmental and behavioral pediatricians, as part of an autism assessment, we do look at using other forms of tools, like the ADOS, to look at some aspects of communication. So while I am not a speech-language pathologist, I am not completely speech-language pathology naive.

Kristofco: You said you were not formally trained as a speech-language pathologist. When is the last time you had any training in speech-language pathology through those rotations that you mentioned?

Ross: Probably when I worked at Einstein. I worked with the speech-language pathologist in our autism program.

Kristofco: And you don't hold any state or governmental licensure as a speech-language pathologist, correct?

Ross: No.

Kristofco: You don't hold yourself out as a speech-language pathologist?

Ross: No.

Kristofco: You don't have a certificate of clinical competence from the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association?

Ross: I don't.

Kristofco: You have not published any peer reviewed research in the field of speech-language pathology?

Ross: No.

Kristofco: And you have not taught any speech-language pathology classes, correct?

Ross: Well, I have presented on speech-language pathology in the context of autism. And it's the role of the developmental and behavioral pediatrician to be familiar with occupational therapy, physical therapy and speech-language pathology in terms of understanding how they interplay with autism.

Kristofco: So does that mean you have not taught any speech-language pathology classes?

Ross: No.

[An ambiguous “No,” but I assume she means “No, I haven’t taught any speech-language pathology classes,” as opposed to “No, I have taught speech-language pathology classes.”]

Kristofco: And the places where you have lectured, you have been brought into a class to lecture about autism, correct?

Ross: Yes.

Kristofco: And you have not been trained in Spelling to Communicate, correct?

Ross: No.

Kristofco: What information did you gather to prepare your expert report?

Ross: I mean, I don't remember when I prepared this, but it looks like, you know, I—I mean, I would have to really look at it. I didn't see this before I came today, and I have not looked at it in a number of years. But it looks like some of it talks about my experience and training, and some of it talks about some of the work that I did. Some of it talks about my experiences with Alex and participating in the community programs and the presentation that we gave together. I guess that's it.

Kristofco: Anything else?

Ross: I mean, it looks like there's some articles. Looks like there is something about stereotype bias here. There is something about epigenetics in here.

[Stereotype bias has been evoked to explain the failure of autistic individuals to retrieve the words needed to describe pictures presented to them during message-passing tests. It was evoked, for example, by Vikram Jaswal in a highly problematic eye-tracking study that purported to find an alternative source of evidence for S2C.]

Kristofco: Did you conduct any examinations of Alex to come to your—the opinions that you have expressed in the report marked as D2?

Ross: That's this? Yeah, I have seen Alex in multiple contexts, both in the community when giving presentations with him and in my office.

Kristofco: When was the last time you examined Alex?

Ross: I mean, prior to him transitioning to Mary Stevens, so it would have been a long time ago.

 [Some clarifying questions about dates ensue.]

Kristofco: It's fair to say that the last time you actually examined Alex was some time prior to April of 2019?

Ross: I think so.

Kristofco: Okay. And did you conduct any testing of Alex to prepare your report?

Ross: A developmental and behavioral pediatrician does not always do testing. Sometimes we interact with the patient.

Kristofco: Is that a “No”?

Ross: There was no formal assessment.

Kristofco: So was there an informal testing that you did of Alex in order to prepare your report?

Ross: Yes, I spoke to him and we interacted with him in that way.

Kristofco: Did you conduct any testing of Spelling to Communicate to prepare your report?

Ross: In having a conversation with him and with his communication partner, which was his mom. I guess that was an informal interaction.

Kristofco: I'm not asking about an interaction. I am asking you whether you conducted any testing of whether the Spelling to Communicate methodology works in order to prepare your report?

Ross: No, I did not do formal testing.

Kristofco: Have you ever conducted any testing of whether Spelling to Communicate works for Alex?

Ross: No.

Kristofco: Did you do any research on whether Spelling to Communicate is a generally accepted methodology in order to prepare your report?

Ross: I would have to look at this report to say exactly what I looked at... I spoke with other people that worked with him, like Susan Chaplick and Anne Robbins and Dr. Ghaffari, to make sure that what I was seeing reflected what they were seeing as well.

[As a reminder, these individuals are:

·         Neuropsychologist Dr. Anne Robbins the person whom A.L.’s parents hired to do an independent, neurocognitive assessment of A.L  that was based, in part, on S2C-generated output.

·         Speech-language pathologist Susan Chaplick, whom A.L.’s parents introduced to S2C several years ago and who accepts S2Ced output as valid, and

·         Child and adolescent psychiatrist Dr. Manley Ghaffari, who testified in an earlier hearing that the letter board was “extremely effective in allowing [A.L.] to express his thoughts and feelings.”

]

Ross: [Continuing] I will say that I think there were articles by Jaswal where he looked and it showed that eye tracking, when people are spelling to communicate, their eyes are looking at the word, at the letters as they and I will say that since then, there are several self-advocates that are spelling to communicate that are part of—that are going to college that are on the self-advocate board of the autism board of the Society of America.

[As suggested by her use of the term “stereotype threat”, Dr. Ross is indeed familiar with Jaswal’s eye tracking study. This highly problematic publication, along with the fact that some people who are subjected to S2C are now attending college or listed as board members of autism organizations, seems, in Dr. Ross’s mind, to count as evidence that S2C is an accepted methodology.]

Kristofco: And that's the research that you did as to whether the Spelling to Communicate was an accepted methodology?

Ross: I looked at the literature that existed at that point, and I continued to look at ongoing literature.

[Apparently that literature did not include the published critiques of Jaswal—i.e., this and this—even though the former was published shortly after Jaswal’s paper came out.]

Kristofco: Now, in your report, you do not give an opinion that Alex has apraxia, correct?

Ross: I may not have.

Kristofco: Okay. In your report, you do not provide any scientific or medical opinion as to why Spelling to Communicate works for Alex, correct?

Ross: I don't know why it works for him. I don't know why lots of things work, but I still believe that it does work for him.

Kristofco: Now, during the due process hearing when you testified, and I can show you the testimony if you would like, but you said, “I cannot say specifically why having somebody beside him holding the board helps him communicate.” Is that still your opinion today?

Ross: I mean, I can hypothesize as to why that's the case. I think it's sort of the case for a lot of us. And when we have—if I had a person at a basketball game that looks to the stands and sees his parents and then makes a layup versus seeing that they are not there and doesn't. You know what I mean? I don't have an actual method. I could hypothesize. But again, I don't know why a lot of things work or how, but I still know that they do.

[This is a comparison I hadn’t heard before.

·         On one hand: an S2C practitioner holding up a letterboard to a minimally-speaking autistic person’s index finger while the person points to letters.

·         On the other hand: parents in the audience of a basketball game being visible to a basketball player who then makes a successful shot.

]

Kristofco: Those hypotheses, you have not tested any of them?

Ross: No.

Kristofco: And you also testified at the due process hearing that, scientifically, “I don't know why this is working for him.” Is that still the case?

Ross: That's still the case. But I also don't know why people have autism, and a lot of people have been studying that. And I don't know why it presents different ways in different people, but it still presents that way.

[Another bizarre comparison:

·         On one hand, the unknowns about what autism is and why it presents itself in different ways in different people.

·         On the other hand, the unknowns about how A.L. has bypassed the limitations of profound autism (which include minimal tuning in to speech and, therefore, minimal opportunities for acquiring language) to become fully literate. Fully literate, that is, not when he “writes things down” on his own, but only when someone holds up a letterboard to his index finger.

Apparently the latter set of unknowns (A.L.’s purported attainment of full literacy) does not strike Dr. Ross as any more mysterious than the former set (what autism is and why it’s heterogeneous).]

Kristofco: If you could turn to page 7 of defendant's exhibit 2. On page 7, you say, “Based on my observations and clinical judgment, the letter board is effective communication for Alex.” It's the second sentence in the first full paragraph there on page 7. Do you see that?

Ross: Mm-hmm.

Kristofco: Now, when you say the letter board is effective communication for Alex, do you mean just the letter board by itself or do you mean the whole Spelling to Communicate methodology?

Ross: I guess for my perspective, they are the same.

Kristofco: Okay. So from your perspective, it's the same thing if you put a letter board on the table in front of Alex versus if somebody holds the board?

Ross: No, I think Alex needs a communication partner.

Kristofco: So your opinion then is that you believe that the letter board with the communication partner is effective communication for Alex?

Ross: Yes.

Kristofco: And what is the definition of an effective communication?

Ross: I don't have a specific definition, but for Alex I will say he can use the letter board and ask for something. And when he gets it, he agrees that's what he wanted and he uses it. So inasmuch as communication is getting your needs met or communicating a thought or, you know—and having a reciprocal conversation with Alex using the letter board, I’ve had a question; he's answered. I've said something based on his answer; he has said something based on what I asked. Do you know what I mean? I feel like it's communication.

[Here we see the some circular reasoning so often resorted to by supporters of S2C, followed by a non-sequitur.

·         The circular reasoning: S2C seems valid because some of the messages it generates communicate that other messages it generates are valid.

·         The non-sequitur: S2C seems valid because the messages it generates function as part of a reciprocal conversation and feel like communication. Of course, messages will seem reciprocal and feel like communication whether they’re controlled by the minimal speaker or by their communication partner.

But Kristofco is about to make a very different point—one I had not anticipated.]

Kristofco: So in using that phrase in your report, you did not mean effective communication as that phrase is used under the Americans with Disabilities Act?

Ross: I'm not completely familiar with exactly how it is used in the Americans with Disabilities Act. But for me, inasmuch as Alex was communicating his thoughts and needs, I felt like he [it?] was.

Kristofco: Why did you use the phrase "effective communication" at all in your report?

Ross: Because that was my perspective.

Kristofco: Okay. Are you aware that to be effective, an aid or a service must be provided in such a way to protect the privacy and independence of the individual with the disability?

[Not just independence, but privacy. I’m reminded of Janyce’s post on Four in the Bedroom. We’re so focused on independence that we probably don’t give privacy the attention it deserves.]

Ross: I was not specifically aware of that nuance, no.

Kristofco: Using the letter board and communication partner, it's true that Alex cannot have a private conversation with his doctor, correct?

Ross: I guess not.

 [This clarifies a question I had about something Dr. Ross said during her direct examination: “...if it's just he and I in my office and his statements tend to be longer, , sometimes I will write it and then I will look at it...” Was Dr. Ross the one holding up the letterboard at A.L.’s appointments with her, I had wondered. Her answer here suggests not. It seems that, during A.L.’s doctor’s visits, his mother-cum-communication partner was always present.]

Kristofco: And using the letter board and communication partner, it's true that Alex is always relying upon the presence of a third person to communicate, correct?

Ross: At this time.

Kristofco: You say, "At this time." Alex has been using Spelling to Communicate over seven years, and he is still not able to communicate independently, correct?

[I hadn’t done the math. Seven years is a very long time.]

Ross: But that's not unlike other people that have used Spelling to Communicate that have taken many, many, many years to communicate independently.

[“Communicate independently.” Among S2C proponents, this is a common fallacy. It amounts to the assumption that having the letterboard/keyboard is on a stationary surface and no one touching the person who types on it is enough to guarantee that the typing is independent.

But most people would agree that truly independent communication includes the ability to produce accurate, unrehearsed answers to spontaneous questions without the same few familiar people hovering within auditory, visual and/or tactile cueing range.

In fact, there is no evidence that anyone who has been subjected to FC or any of its variants has ever gotten to truly independent communication that comes anywhere near the linguistic sophistication of their FC-generated messages.

What is true is that some individuals, after many years of FC/RPM/S2C, get to a point where those auditory, visual, and/or tactile cues are so subtle that many naïve observers, including autism experts, can’t see them.

And what’s also true is that some of the individuals who have been subjected to FC/RPM/S2C are able to produce lengthy rehearsed and memorized letter sequences (some individuals with profound autism have demonstrated tremendous rote-memorization skills), possibly without any cueing. See, for example, Tito Mukhopadhyay typing out a 154-letter poem on this segment of 60 Minutes.]

Ross: [Continuing] And Alex has also had a lot of other health issues, including incapacitating migraine headaches and other issues. And there has also been COVID, which has impacted a lot of people.

Kristofco: Just to make sure the record is clear, Alex has been using Spelling to Communicate for over seven years, and he is still not able to communicate independently, correct?

Ross: That's my understanding.

Kristofco: And isn't it true that the opinion that you provide in your report, that this letter board and communication partner is effective communication for Alex is based exclusively on the fact that you have seen it and based on what you have seen you believe that it works?

Ross: It's also based on my interactions with the other providers that care for him. And when I see it, there is no nobody moving the board, there is nobody touching him. So I don't know whywhat else could explain that he is spelling words and sentences.

[The fact that Dr. Ross isn’t aware of any board movements doesn’t mean they don’t happen: training in cue detection probably didn’t figure into any of her coursework or practicums, extensive and impressive though these were.]

Kristofco: Who are the other providers that you mentioned?

Ross: Dr. Ghaffari, Susan Chaplick, Anne Robbins.

Kristofco: So because the other people believe it works, that bolstered your belief that it works?

Ross: It's not that it bolsters, but it's helpful. I mean, that's not my opinion, it's not based on their opinion. But when providers generally agree independently, that's sort of nice.

Kristofco: Well, let's just talk about what your opinion is based on. Is your opinion based on anything other than the fact that you have seen it in action and you believe it works?

Ross: Yes, that's what it's based on. I have seen it in action and I believe it works.

Kristofco: Would you agree with me that there's a difference between science and belief?

Ross: Sure.

Kristofco: And what facts or data is the opinion that you have given in your report about effective communication based upon?

Ross: Well, when Alex touches the letters and no one is touching him and nobody is touching the board, he spells words. He functionally answered questions that I asked him. He conveyed things about himself that other people did not know. He was able to request things and indicate that requesting them has led to him getting what he wants when people listen. And also, his behaviors changed because he was not quite as frustrated. Not every day, not on the whole, but to some degree based on his communication.

[As I noted earlier, the only specific example we have of Alex “conveying things about himself that other people did not know” is the ear infection story. In general, facilitators (especially if they are parents) can intuit what their client wants, especially if it’s something rewarding and/or predictable. And with autism, where “insistence on sameness” is one of the diagnostic symptoms, much of what’s rewarding or upsetting is highly predictable.]

Kristofco: Is it fair to say that everything that you discussed about Alex in your report from 2017 on, you learned through him communicating with the letter board and communication partner?

Ross: I think so. I don't really—it's hard to put myself—it was a number of years ago. I would not say everything, because I do also get information from other people. But I believe in terms of his communication that that was him.

Kristofco: You would agree with me that the basis for your opinion, the fact that you saw and believe it works, is something that is not testable, correct?

Ross: I think that might be true, but I think a lot of things are not testable.

Kristofco: So, on page 6 of your report, it's the first sentence of the second paragraph. You say, “When Alex began to communicate with a letter board and communication partner, I looked for any possible way that this might not be his own voice and found none.” Do you see that?

[Some clarification about where exactly in the paragraph that was.]

Kristofco: I will read the sentence again. It said, ‘When Alex began to communicate with the letter board and communication partner, I looked for any possible way that this might not be his own voice and found none.” Do you see that?

Ross: Yes.

Kristofco: What specifically did you look for?

Ross: I looked for somebody touching him, somebody moving the board, somebody saying letters that he was not pointing to.

Kristofco: Did you conduct a message passing test?

Ross: No.

Kristofco: Did you show Alex a picture that the communication partner could not see and then ask Alex questions about the picture?

Ross: No, but it's not my job to undermine his level of communication. I have a therapeutic rapport with him, so it wasn't that I was testing him, that would have eroded the trust. I was watching him. Nobody was moving the board, nobody was touching him, and he was touching the letters that were spelling the words. I don't test my patients to discredit them or to sort of express a disbelief in what they are doing or not doing. So that would be the purpose of doing a message passing test, and I don't know that there would have been a purpose or need to have done that. Based on what I saw, I felt convinced he was communicating.

[Here they are once again: the circular reasoning and the certainty that observation is reliable.

The latter is common enough to have a name: naïve realism. Presumably Dr. Ross would know not to apply this line of reasoning at a magic show, where she presumably wouldn’t conclude that rabbits can be conjured out of hats just because it looks that way.

As for circular reasoning, here it manifests itself in the notion that any test that assesses whether A.L. is generating the messages should be avoided because such a test would express disbelief in A.L. as a message-generator.]

Kristofco: So is it fair to say that for the purposes for which you were treating Alex, it was not in your interest to challenge whether this system actually worked?

Ross: I don't really think that's fair to say, actually. My goal is not to challenge my patients, it's to observe them. I use my 25 years of clinical experience on what is happening and my interrelationship with people from other disciplines to look at what is happening. And based on what I was seeing, I felt like he was communicating and I didn't really feel a need to sort of say, “Oh, Alex, I doubt you. I'm going to try to prove that you are not communicating.” It just didn't seem—

Kristofco: Well, if—

Ross: —meaningful to do that.

Kristofco: Sorry. If this isn't truly Alex's voice coming through the letter board, isn't that something that you would want to know?

Ross: I felt like it was his voice based on my experience and watching him. So I do feel like I know that, first of all. And second of all, my greater fear would be to say that somebody who is communicating is not communicating. To silence somebody by saying that they are not communicating and going after them as if I’m going to doubt you and I’m going to prove that you are not communicating, that's not a perspective that a clinician takes. And I would be terrified if somebody were actually communicating, even if it were one person, by using this letter board, I would be terrified of silencing them. I would think that that would be fairly horrifying.

[For Dr. Ross, the prospect of “silencing” someone by displaying doubt about whether it’s them communicating is more terrifying than the prospect that it’s not them communicating—and that their communications and very personhood are (however unwittingly) being hijacked and supplanted by their facilitator.]

Kristofco: And it would be worth it to allow people who were not really communicating to continue to believe so just so you didn't silence that one person?

Ross: Well, in my experience, the people who it did not work for did not continue it.

[This takes us back to the faulty implication that arose during direct examination: namely, that because Dr. Ross (unlike most S2C promoters) acknowledges that S2C doesn’t work for everyone, and because she has observed individuals for whom S2C doesn’t work, she must therefore be capable of accurately distinguishing the successes from the failures.

And we see here that, with the certainty that she can tell when S2C doesn’t work, and the certainty that everyone she knows for whom S2C didn’t work has stopped using it, Dr. Ross is apparently at peace with the absence of any message-passing assessments in A.L.’s case—or, presumably, in the case of any of her patients.]

Kristofco: I guess it's fair to say that you didn't ask Alex a question outside the presence of the communication partner and see if Alex could answer the question when the person came back?

Ross: No, there would be no purpose. That would be set up to sort of say to him, “I don't believe you are communicating, so I am going to try to trick you to see if this is real.” That's just not what a clinician does to a patient.

Kristofco: How is that tricking him?

Ross: It's not tricking him, but it's sort of testing. It would be like “I am going to set you up and see if this is real.”

[More circular reasoning: the test is only a testing of A.L.’s communications if A.L. is the one generating the communications. But the whole point of the test is to determine whether the person Dr. Ross thinks she is talking to and is afraid of offending really is A.L.]

Ross: [Continuing] From what I was seeing, it felt real. So to put him in a situation where the point–the perspective was, “I doubt you,” didn't seem–didn't even occur to me because I was convinced from watching him that it was his communication. So it would not have occurred to me to do that, because I was watching him, and it felt like spontaneous, real communication.

Kristofco: Isn't testing part of the scientific method?

Ross: Yes, but I was not a scientist studying Alex, I was his clinician.

Kristofco: I guess it's fair to say that the method that you used to arrive at your opinion, the, “I have seen it and based on my observation I believe it works,” that methodology has not been peer reviewed, correct?

Ross: Well, I think there are more studies of people using a letter board or Spelling to Communicate. And more and more—we are seeing more and more data to support it as a form of functional communication. Not for everyone, but for a lot of people.

[Vague allusions to “more and more studies” that supposedly support FC and its variants—we keep encountering this kind of hand-waving by S2C supporters. But when they cite specific examples, it’s always the same problematic studies: Jaswal et al.’s eye tracking study (critiqued here and here), and Jaswal et al.’s subsequent literacy study (critiqued here) and “studies out Cambridge University,” which aren’t actually studies, but rather a research proposal that has yet to go anywhere.]

Kristofco: That was not my question. My question was: the methodology that you used to arrive at your opinion, that methodology of, “I have seen it and I believe that it works based on my observation,” that has not been peer reviewed?

Ross: It is—that's what doctors do. I mean, that's what doctors do. They use whole body, listening to watch what patients do. So even if somebody had some kind of a medical condition, you watch how they are moving their body; you listen to what they are saying; you might even watch the people around him, because the truth is, we are kind of interdependent. So you wouldn't [would?] incorporate the perspective of somebody else. I don't know why somebody with a developmental disability should be expected to be less interdependent than the rest of us.

[This is not the first time I’ve seen people invoke vague notions of “holistic” assessments to bypass requests for solid evidence and precise measurements: this happens routinely throughout the education world. Nor is it the first time I’ve seen FC/S2C proponents invoke vague notions of how we’re all interdependent to anticipate/dismiss concerns about facilitator dependence.]

Ross: [Continuing] But that's a part of the art of medicine. That's what you do. And you don't test it scientifically every time you see a patient. You have experience and background, and that's what you do.

 [It strikes me that, for many clinicians, it’s all about observation. And while clinical observation may work well in most situations, the moment an FC/S2C communication partner enters the scene, with all the subtle cueing that this inevitably introduces, everything changes. Most clinicians are neither primed nor trained to look for facilitator cueing. Their eyes, quite naturally, focus exclusively on their clients; not on the person holding up the letterboard board. The five clinicians involved in this case—two speech-language pathologists, a child and adolescent psychiatrist, a neuropsychologist, and a developmental pediatrician—are no exception.

Put another way: with medical symptoms, behavioral symptoms, or symptoms pertaining to spoken language use and comprehension, clinical judgments are appropriate. But where FC-generated communications are concerned, no one should be relying on judgments by clinicians.]

Kristofco: The observation piece that a doctor uses is part of an overall evaluation of a patient, correct?

Ross: Yes.

Kristofco: So you gather actual data when you examine the patients. You might test their body by pressing on parts of it; you might take their temperature; you might take their blood pressure; you might take tests, like x-rays or some other form of examination. But the observation part that you are talking about is part of the overall examination process, correct?

Ross: The practice of medicine is an art. It's not just based on scientific data like labs or numbers or signs.

[The practice of medicine as an art—as with holistic assessments, there’s a warm and cuddly feeling here. But when medical practice as an art and medical practice as a science come into conflict, I think most of us, when push comes to shove, would prefer for science to prevail. If the blood pressure monitor shows 150 but my doctor thinks I look perfectly fine, should I not worry?]

Kristofco: Are you familiar with the fact that the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association has come out with a statement warning against the use of the methodology that Alex uses?

Ross: Yeah. I think it's horrible.

[That’s quite a harsh judgment against what’s actually a rather carefully worded warning:

It is the position of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) that use of the Rapid Prompting Method (RPM) is not recommended because of prompt dependency and the lack of scientific validity. Furthermore, information obtained through the use of RPM should not be assumed to be the communication of the person with a disability.

]

Kristofco: And you are aware, of course, that that is the nationwide association for speech-language pathologists, correct?

Ross: Yes. And I’m also aware that Barry Prizant, who has been given their highest honor, also disagrees with them. And many of us who are clinicians don't think that blanket statement should be made about all people under all circumstances. So I don't know what to say, but I disagree with ASHA, as do many other people.

[Barry Prizant, another of the plaintiff’s expert witnesses, is the highest profile speech-language pathologist to support S2C—and another example of a clinician who cites clinical observations as the basis for S2C’s validity. Indeed, Prizant joined a 2018 letter campaign against the ASHA position statement, writing “[I]n no, and I mean NO instances were they physically or gesturally directed to specific letter targets.”

It’s true that Prizant was honored by ASHA—back in 2014, four years prior to going public in support of S2C. These days, Prizant frequently promotes S2C on his Uniquely Human Podcast.]

Kristofco: So the fact that that statement has been issued, and the fact that you have several people who are—several patients who are using this methodology, none of this has led you to believe that you should, perhaps, test it to see if it works?

[Let’s pause to do the math. Presumably these several patients of Dr. Ross’s who are subjected to S2C are patients for whom she has deemed it to work. Combine these with the several patients of hers for whom she’s deemed S2C not to work, and we have to wonder how much of an S2C-magnet Dr. Ross’s practice has evolved into. Is she the main developmental pediatrician in the Philadelphia area with patients who are subjected to S2C? Are there others out there that are flying under the radar because they haven’t (yet) been called as expert witnesses in trials against school districts?]

Ross: I believe when I watch somebody, even though somebody is holding the board, if the board is not being moved and no one is touching them, I don't—I mean, to me that's enough evidence that it's working. And I don't understand how an organization like ASHA could make a global statement about a whole group, a heterogenous group no less, and not look at them one by one. I think no responsible clinician or medical professional or other person would make a decision on a person without seeing or interacting with them personally.

[ASHA’s position statement clearly pertains to methodologies: it makes no global statements about any groups. Dr. Ross’s claim that ASHA’s statement is about groups rather than methodologies is akin to her claim that message-passing tests are being about the person being facilitated as opposed to the methodology to which that person is being subjected.

As for Dr. Ross’s statement that decisions about individual people should include observations and interactions—that’s perfectly reasonable. But given the widely recognized concerns about facilitator influence in FC/RPM/S2C, it’s also perfectly reasonable to do a message-passing test of the methodology in every specific instance of its use. Indeed, given the high stakes involved—personhood and communication rights—it would be highly unreasonable not to do a message-passing test of the methodology in every specific instance of its use.]

Mr.Kristofco: I don't have any additional questions, your honor.

Thus concludes the cross-examination of Dr. Wendy Ross.

For me, the key takeaways are:

·         Dr. Ross’s implications that clinical observation trumps formal testing and that holistic assessments trump scientific measurements.

·         Her assumption that as long as her own observations tell her that “the board is not being moved and no one is touching him,” A.L.’s S2C-generated output must represent messages coming from A.L. rather than from his communication partner.

·         Her repeated resort to circular reasoning to justify S2C and to explain why it wouldn’t be appropriate for her to conduct a message-passing test.

·         How she seems more concerned about falsely accusing someone of not communicating than about the possibility that this person’s communication is being hijacked, and his personhood suppressed, however unwittingly, by someone else.

In my next post, I’ll return to the subjects of board movements, message-passing tests, and what’s more ethical than what. Stay tuned for the examination (and cross-examination) of the communication partner designated by the plaintiffs to facilitate A.L.’s testimony at the prospective trial.